Psychiatry Investig Search

CLOSE


Psychiatry Investig > Volume 21(2); 2024 > Article
Park, Chang, Hong, Kim, Park, Jung, Kim, Hahm, and An: The Effect of Time Spent on Online Gaming on Problematic Game Use in Male: Moderating Effects of Loneliness, Living Alone, and Household Size

Abstract

Objective

This study aimed to investigate the association between gaming time and problematic game use (PGU) within a large sample of Korean male gamers and to examine the potential moderating effects of loneliness, living alone, and household size.

Methods

This study employed data from 743 male gamers from the National Mental Health Survey 2021, a nationally representative survey of mental illness conducted in South Korea. Self-reported data on the average gaming time per day, severity of PGU, loneliness, living alone, and household size were used.

Results

Gaming time was positively associated with PGU and this relationship was significantly moderated by loneliness such that the positive effect of gaming time on PGU was greater when the levels of loneliness were high. The three-way interaction effect of gaming time, loneliness, and living alone was also significant, in that the moderating effect of loneliness on the relationship between gaming time and PGU was significant only in the living alone group. However, household size (i.e., number of housemates) did not moderate the interaction between gaming time and loneliness among gamers living with housemates.

Conclusion

These results suggest the importance of considering loneliness and living arrangements of male gamers, in addition to gaming time, in identifying and intervening with individuals at heightened risk of PGU.

INTRODUCTION

The popularization of smartphones and the growth of the mobile gaming have increased the variety and availability of video games [1], leading to a surge in global gaming [2], particularly in countries like Korea with high internet penetration [3]. In 2021, the game usage rate of Koreans was 71.3%, and among game users, the mobile game usage rate (90.9%) highly exceeded the PC game (57.6%) or console game (21.0%) usage rate [4]. As the number of game players increase, and the media and content of games become more diversified, game use has surged not only as a form of amusement but also as an alternative way to fulfill various psychological needs, such as social engagement, accomplishment, emotional avoidance, and stress release [5]. This may be more evident in online and multiplayer games with social components [6]. The number of online gamers has exceeded that of offline games, and this gap has widened during the coronavirus disease-2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, when offline activities were highly restricted [7,8].
However, overuse of games may have negative effects on users, in the form of compromised physical/mental health, academic/professional performance, and interpersonal relationships as a result of failure to control their game use and their tendency to prioritize gaming over other duties/activities [9,10]. Such tendencies are referred to as problematic gaming or problematic game use (PGU) [6,11], and if they persist over an extended period (e.g., more than 12 months), they can be diagnosed as a gaming disorder in the International Classification of Diseases 11th Revision [12]. Although the prevalence of gaming disorder varies considerably across regions, it is known to be particularly high in East Asia, including Korea [13]. In addition, although controversial, some studies have argued that increased PGU severity may cause neurological changes similar to those in substance use disorders [10,14].
From this perspective, chronic PGU is often considered a subtype of behavioral addiction, underscoring the importance of early screening and intervention to minimize psychological and socioeconomic damage. To achieve this goal, it is essential to identify risk factors and reveal how they may conjointly have varying effects on individuals with PGU vulnerability. Male gamers can be considered an important target population for this endeavor, given that PGU is generally more prevalent in male than in female. According to a recent meta-analysis, male are more than twice as likely to experience PGU as female [15]. In addition, several studies have shown that male and female also differ in major risk factors of PGU and in the relationship between the factors [16,17]. These findings suggest that an approach to investigate male and female separately may be useful in studying the underlying mechanisms of PGU.
Excessive gaming is a major risk factor for PGU. However, it is important to note that quantitative indicators (e.g., time and frequency of engagement) of digital media use may only partially correlate with problematic use [18,19], and this may also be the case for PGU [20,21]. Latent profile analysis studies conducted in Europe and Asia have consistently identified two subgroups within gamers, with differences in PGU severity but no significant differences in gaming time [22,23]; that is, those who have socio-psychological issues and those who do not may be differentiated, even among individuals who commit a great deal of time to gaming. Heavy gamers who are not maladjusted and are actively engaged in social activities, such as gaming YouTubers and professional gamers, exemplify that excessive gaming does not always lead to PGU, which is also indirectly supported by the ineffectiveness of gaming control legislations in many Asian countries, which focus on restricting usage time [24,25]. This evidence suggests that examining gaming time alone may not be sufficient to assess PGU risk, and that it may be necessary to investigate the conditions under which excessive game use leads to PGU.
Loneliness is a subjective emotional experience of perceived isolation that is independent of the actual lack of social relationships [26], and may function as a “railroad switch” connecting excessive gaming with PGU, especially in male. Studies have found that loneliness shows a weak or no significant relationship with game usage but nevertheless demonstrates a consistent positive correlation with PGU [27-29]. In addition, according to Stockdale and Coyne’s research comparing problematic gamers and low-risk gamers, the two groups did not vary in perceived levels of social support networks (e.g., the number of regular interactions), but the PGU group experienced more feelings of social isolation such as loneliness [30]. While evidence is still scarce, some researchers have suspected that loneliness may reduce responsiveness to positive social interactions (which is a protective factor for PGU), resulting in mistrust of others and social withdrawal [31]. In a similar context, gamers who suffer severe loneliness tend to have relatively low social competence and/or entertainment sought motivation (e.g., playing games to interact with friends or having fun) and high escape and fantasy motivation (e.g., using games to escape the real world or break away from their usual identity) [17,28].
Interestingly, both the main and indirect effects (through game use motivations) of loneliness on PGU were found to be significant only in male [17]. This differential effect of loneliness by gender is consistent with previous studies on other addictive behaviors (e.g., pathological gambling and problematic social media use) [32,33]. Considering that the game market tends to be more tuned towards men’s preferences [34], and self-efficacy about the game is generally higher in male [35], the above result may reflect that for female, playing games has a relatively lower incentive than other behavioral options and is not expected to be a useful tool for addressing their loneliness.
Additionally, we explored the possibility that the potential moderating effect of loneliness on the association between gaming time and PGU differed depending on the presence or absence of a housemate. As stated above, because loneliness is an emotion based on subjective experience, individuals may experience severe loneliness even if they live with others. Indeed, it has been suggested that loneliness and living arrangements should be considered as close but independent factors that may determine individual outcomes [36,37], and previous studies have shown that their correlation is inconsistent and influenced by factors such as age and relationship quality with housemates [36-38]. Nevertheless, the presence of housemates may serve as a minimal safeguard against PGU resulting from loneliness and excessive game use. In addition to monitoring maladaptive digital media use [39], housemates may prevent loneliness from lasting too long by decreasing the time spent completely alone [38]. The presence of a housemate may also mitigate the risk of other mental health issues such as depression and stress, which can increase vulnerability to PGU [40,41].
However, even if the risk of PGU is lower in households with housemates than in solitary ones, it is uncertain whether this effect changes as a function of household size (i.e., the number of housemates). Several studies have found that persons living with more than three or four housemates are more likely to feel lonely [42], perceive external information more negatively [43], and be more susceptible to environmental stress than those living with fewer housemates [44]. Living with too many housemates can lead to decreased social support, privacy invasion, limited alone time, and more negative interactions [44,45]. Therefore, we believe that the possible role of household size in PGU should be examined from a more exploratory perspective. Nevertheless, given that gaming is mostly done at home, regardless of age [4], we anticipated that the greater the number of housemates who can continually monitor and moderate gaming behavior, the less likely lonely game overusers are to experience PGU.
Based on these findings, we hypothesized the following:
H1. The relationship between gaming time and PGU is moderated by loneliness. In other words, the positive effect of gaming time on PGU is greater when loneliness is high.
H2. The interaction effect between gaming time and loneliness on PGU differs depending on whether participants live with housemate(s). In other words, the moderating effect of loneliness on the relationship between gamers’ gaming time and PGU is greater for individuals living alone.
H3. Among participants living with housemate(s), household size moderates the interaction between gaming time and loneliness. Specifically, the moderating effect of loneliness on the relationship between gaming time and PGU is greater for gamers living with fewer housemates.

METHOD

Participants and procedures

This study used data from the National Mental Health Survey 2021 conducted by the Ministry of Health and Welfare in South Korea. In this survey, 5,511 participants were recruited from all regions of the country using a random stratification sampling method, and trained surveyors collected data through face-to-face interviews. To be included in the analyses of the study, participants had to be male gamers who reported currently playing online/internet games, had a non-zero average daily gaming time, and were willing to answer questions about housemates. Based on these inclusion criteria, 743 participants were selected.

Measures

PGU

The Game Overuse Screening Questionnaire (GOS-Q) was used to assess PGU severity based on the respondents’ experiences over the past month. It is a self-reported measure with 30 items (e.g., “I tried to reduce the amount of time I play games, but it is difficult”). Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“never”) to 4 (“almost always”). Higher scores reflected more severe PGU, and a total score exceeding 38.5 is an indicator of high-risk. 46 The GOS-Q has been validated with good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α=0.96) [46]. In this study, the alpha value was also 0.96.

Gaming time

Participants were asked whether they had been playing games recently and if they had, they were asked to report their daily game usage time on weekdays and weekends in minutes. For this study, the average daily gaming time was computed by adding five times the weekday usage time and two times the weekend usage time, and dividing it by 7.

Loneliness

The Loneliness and Social Isolation Scale (LSIS) is a 6-item self-reported measure consisting of three subscales: social support, social networks, and loneliness [26]. In the current analysis, we used the loneliness subscale, which comprised two items (“I feel lonely.,” “I feel isolated.”). Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (“never”) to 3 (“always”), with higher scores reflecting higher levels of loneliness over the past month. In a previous study, the Cronbach’s alpha for the LSIS loneliness subscale was 0.81. In this study, it was 0.85 [26].

Living alone

Participants provided information about who they were currently living with, at home. Living alone was a binary variable, such that if the respondent was living with another person, regardless of the number of housemates, it was coded as “living together”; otherwise, it was coded as “living alone.”

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics version 28.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine the demographic characteristics of the sample, descriptive statistics, and bivariate correlations. Model 1 of the PROCESS macro for SPSS (version 4.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was then used to test the two-way interaction effect of gaming time and loneliness on PGU (H1). Thereafter, using Model 3 of the PROCESS macro, a three-way interaction effect of gaming time, loneliness, and living alone on PGU was tested (H2). Finally, we performed the aforementioned analyses again for household size instead of living alone using data from participants who had housemate(s) (H3). Continuous variables were mean centered to minimize multicollinearity [47]. To ensure that the results are not significantly affected by the distribution of the variables, the bootstrap method was applied [48], and the number of resamples was set to 5,000. Given that age has been suggested to be related to PGU levels [49,50], we have included it as a covariate in all analyses to control for its effects.

Ethics

The first author’s University’s Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved this study (IRB No. SKKU 2022-08-018). At the time of the survey, all procedures were conducted in compliance with ethical procedures monitored by government agencies, and participants filled informed consent forms, including consent for the future use of data for research purposes.

RESULTS

Sample characteristics

Demographic characteristics are summarized in Table 1. The age range of the participants was 18 to 72 years (Mage= 34.62, SDage=10.84), and the majority (89.4%) were under the age of 50. In terms of employment, 504 (67.8%) participants were full-time workers, 100 (13.5%) were temporary workers, and 139 (18.7%) were unemployed or were students. Regarding the level of education, 11 (1.5%) graduated from middle school or lower, 280 (37.7%) graduated from high school, 451 (60.7%) graduated from college or had higher degrees, and 1 (0.1%) did not respond. A total of 194 (26.1%) participants were classified as high-risk for PGU based on the cutoff points, and 148 (19.9%) reported living alone. Of those living with housemates, 137 (18.4% of the total) had one housemate, 235 (31.6% of the total) had two, 190 (25.6% of the total) had three, and 33 (4.4% of the total) had four or more.

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations are presented in Table 2. The average daily gaming time of the participants was 1.98 hours (approximately 1 hour and 59 min, SD=1.50). The range was 0.29 to 9.43 hours, but 65.9% of participants reported 2 hours or less per day and 85.1% reported 3 hours or less. The mean scores for PGU and loneliness were 36.96 (SD=10.09) and 0.81 (SD=1.27), respectively. In addition, the mean household size was 2.77 (SD=1.19). PGU was negatively correlated with age (r=-0.155, p<0.001) and positively correlated with loneliness (r=0.211, p<0.001) and gaming time (r=0.241, p<0.001). This indicates that younger individuals, those experiencing higher levels of loneliness, or those who spent more time playing games were more likely to experience PGU. The correlation between age and gaming time was also significant (r=-0.207, p<0.001). In addition, loneliness was positively correlated with age (r=0.162, p<0.001) and living alone (r=0.158, p<0.001), and negatively correlated with household size (r=-0.119, p=0.001), indicating that younger participants, those who lived alone, or those with fewer housemates were more likely to feel lonely.

Main and interaction effects of gaming time and loneliness on PGU

PROCESS macro Model 1 was used to test the moderating effect of loneliness on the relationship between gaming time and PGU. As presented in Table 3 and Figure 1, the main effects of gaming time (B=1.268, 95% confidence interval [CI]=[0.797-1.739]) and loneliness (B=1.735, 95% CI=[1.184-2.286]) and their interaction effects (B=0.325, 95% CI=[0.031-0.619]) were significant. The model explained 12.4% of the total PGU variance (F [4,738]=26.184, p<0.001).
To probe the two-way interaction, the conditional effect of gaming time on PGU was analyzed by dividing the groups based on the mean and ±1 SD points of loneliness. The positive effect of gaming time on PGU was significant, regardless of the level of loneliness, but the effect for the condition with high loneliness (B=1.679, 95% CI=[1.119-2.240]) was stronger than the average (B=1.268, 95% CI=[0.797-1.739]) or low (B=1.005, 95% CI=[0.451-1.559]) loneliness conditions.

Main and interaction effects of gaming time, loneliness, and living alone on PGU

PROCESS macro Model 3 was used to examine the three-way interaction effects of game time, loneliness, and living alone on PGU. As shown in Table 4, the main effects of gaming time (B=0.805, 95% CI=[0.297-1.312]) and loneliness (B=2.199, 95% CI=[1.556-2.842]) were significant. In terms of interaction effects, as shown in Figure 2, the two-way interaction effects of gaming time×living alone (B=2.718, 95% CI=[1.497-3.940]) and loneliness×living alone (B=-1.359, 95% CI=[-2.561--0.157]), and the three-way interaction effect of gaming time×loneliness×living alone (B=0.757, 95% CI=[0.039-1.475]) on PGU were both statistically significant. The model explained 16.1% of the total PGU variance (F [8,734]=17.567, p<0.001).
To probe the three-way interaction, the conditional effect of the interaction term of gaming time×loneliness on PGU was analyzed by dividing groups based on living arrangements (living alone vs. living together). The moderating effect of loneliness on the relationship between gaming time and PGU was significant only for participants living alone (B=0.900, F [1,734]=7.617, p=0.006) and not for those living with housemate(s) (B=0.143, F [1,734]=0.756, p=0.386). Furthermore, as shown in Figure 3, in the living alone group, gaming time positively predicted PGU at all levels of loneliness, but the effect for the condition of high loneliness (B=4.663, 95% CI=[3.402-5.923]) was larger than the average (B=3.523, 95% CI=[2.408-4.637]) or low (B=2.796, 95% CI=[1.489-4.103]) loneliness conditions.

Main and interaction effects of gaming time, loneliness, and household size on PGU

Using data from participants living with housemate(s) (n=595), we tested the three-way interaction effect once again by entering the household size variable into the model in place of the variable of living alone. The results are presented in Table 5. The main effects of gaming time (B=0.826, 95% CI=[0.294-1.355]) and loneliness (B=2.173, 95% CI=[1.519-2.827]) were significant, but none of the interaction effects, for either the two- or three-way interaction, were significant. The model explained 11.9% of the total PGU variance (F [8,586]=9.923, p<0.001).

DISCUSSION

This study aimed to investigate the underlying mechanism of male gamers’ PGU in the context of increased social concerns regarding problematic digital media use during the COVID-19 pandemic. Specifically, we examined whether the relationship between excessive gaming and PGU differs based on the level of loneliness and whether this interaction effect changes depending on the presence or absence of a housemate or household size. The results indicated that the relationship between gaming time and PGU was moderated by loneliness, such that the positive effect of gaming time on PGU in male gamers was greater when the level of loneliness was high. In addition, the interaction effect of gaming time and loneliness on PGU differed depending on whether the participants lived with housemate(s). The moderating effect of loneliness on the relationship between gaming time and PGU was significant only for participants living alone. Consequently, Hypotheses 1 and 2 of this study were supported. However, Hypothesis 3 was not supported. In other words, household size did not moderate the interaction between gaming time and loneliness among male gamers living with housemates.
As expected, loneliness seems to serve as a “railroad switch” that drives intense gaming to become associated with PGU. Compared to less lonely gamers, lonely male gamers may show a steeper rise in the likelihood of reporting PGU symptoms, such as dysfunction or loss of control, as game usage time increases. This could be attributed to factors such as wanting to ‘escape’ from the real world and identity crises [17,28], which are known to increase vulnerability to PGU [21,41].
However, the role of housemates in the relationship between loneliness, gaming time, and PGU seems complex. Our findings show that the presence of housemates may be a protective factor that decreases the risk of PGU among lonely gamers with excessive gaming time. For gamers living with housemates, the level of loneliness did not change the relationship between gaming time and PGU, although it increased the risk of PGU. Moreover, the presence of housemates seems to buffer the effect of excessive game use on PGU. This is probably because housemates can prevent gamers from being left alone for too long [38], and monitor their behavior [39]. By doing so, housemates can help gamers maintain certain levels of daily function, even if they spend excessive time playing games. Contrary to our expectations, in the living together group, having a greater number of housemates did not change the moderating effect of loneliness in the relationship between gaming time and PGU, due to the potential conflicts arising from having more people in the household [44,45], which can offset the positive effects of having housemates.
Interestingly, as shown in Table 4, the effect of loneliness is rather buffered by living alone. We conducted a supplementary analysis to clarify this relationship. The results of PROCESS macro Model 1 revealed that the effect of loneliness on PGU was significant only in the living together group (B=2.070, 95% CI=[1.415-2.725]) and not in the living alone group (B=0.539, 95% CI=[-0.488-1.566]). This suggests that individuals who experience high levels of loneliness despite living with housemates may have PGU vulnerabilities that cannot be explained by excessive game use alone. Even if gaming time is somewhat modulated by the presence of housemates, lonely gamers may repeatedly experience loss of self-control or negative consequences while immersing themselves in the game whenever they have a chance. In other words, loneliness seems to play an important role in increasing male gamers’s risk of PGU, regardless of living arrangements, but may show group differences in underlying mechanisms (in particular, whether loneliness produces a synergistic effect with excessive game use) or symptom patterns. Thus, it could be useful to investigate whether there are differences in the PGU patterns of male gamers, depending on the presence of housemates.
This study has a few limitations. First, it was impossible to evaluate the direction of causality in the relationship between the variables using a cross-sectional design. For example, although our model posits that loneliness precedes PGU, PGU may also be a risk factor for loneliness due to increased social isolation. Second, due to data constraints, we were unable to analyze the effects of potential factors that may be involved in the PGU mechanism, such as mood symptoms, gaming media, and preferred genre [10,16,17,20,29,41,51]. Future studies that include these factors may further clarify the relationship between the variables. The generalizability of findings is also a concern. Our results, obtained from a Korean sample, may have been influenced by the country’s sociocultural characteristics. In addition, the criteria for PGU vary slightly across scales, as evidenced by the inconsistent terminology (e.g., game addiction, gaming disorder, internet gaming disorder). Thus, replication is needed to confirm if the findings are consistent in other cultural contexts or when using different PGU scales. Furthermore, we cannot exclude the possibility that the relationship between playing time and PGU, and the factors involved, may vary in certain segments of gaming time (e.g., differences in gaming time may no longer be predictive of PGU risk within individuals above a certain level of gaming time), and future studies should examine the relationship between variables in a more systematic and multifaceted manner, for example, using nonlinear regression or latent profile analysis.
Despite these limitations, this study expands the literature on PGU and offers practical implications for developing intervention strategies to address the increasing problems in modern society. Our findings suggest that loneliness, living arrangements, and gaming time may be important factors to consider when screening high-risk individuals for PGU as well as potential targets for intervention. For example, among male who live alone, the level of loneliness can be a useful criterion for determining the risk status of PGU among game overusers. However, the importance of usage time in screening and intervention may be relatively low in male gamers who feel lonely despite living with housemates. In such cases, it would be better to pay more attention to other symptoms of PGU (e.g., impaired self-control, high priority given to gaming, and functional impairments) rather than focusing on excessive game use. Intervention strategies that can be applied to both groups may include alleviating loneliness, exploring adaptive coping strategies, and creating alternative social support systems. The acquisition of adaptive coping strategies for negative affect such as loneliness is an important component of cognitive behavioral therapy, and its effectiveness on PGU has been proven in previous studies [52]. Recently, online interventions based on digital technology have been proposed as an alternative for reducing loneliness and related mental health problems [53,54]. Our findings provide empirical evidence supporting this direction of intervention.

Notes

Availability of Data and Material

The datasets analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to regulations of the National Center for Mental Health (NCMH) of Korea, but can be requested from the NCMH [https://mhs.ncmh.go.kr/menu.es?mid=a10402010000], exclusively for academic research or policy design purposes.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Kyeongwoo Park, Hyein Chang. Data curation: Jin Pyo Hong, Sohee Park, Bong-Jin Hahm, Ji Hyun An. Formal analysis: Kyeongwoo Park, Hyein Chang. Funding acquisition: Jin Pyo Hong, Bong-Jin Hahm. Investigation: Jin Pyo Hong, Bong-Jin Hahm, Ji Hyun An. Methodology: Kyeongwoo Park, Hyein Chang. Project administration: Jin Pyo Hong, Bong-Jin Hahm. Resources: Jin Pyo Hong, Sohee Park, Ji Hyun An. Software: Kyeongwoo Park, Hyein Chang. Supervision: Hyein Chang, Ji Hyun An. Visualization: Kyeongwoo Park. Writing—original draft: Kyeongwoo Park, Hyein Chang. Writing—review & editing: Myung Hyun Kim, Jin Young Jung, Dahae Kim, Ji Hyun An.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Korea Healthcare Technology R&D project, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Republic of Korea (HL19C0018).

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study utilized data of the National Mental Health Survey 2021 (NMHSK-10). The results of this study is irrelevant with the Ministry of Health and Welfare of South Korea.

Figure 1.
Moderation effect of loneliness on the relationship between gaming time and PGU. The model included age as a covariate. *p<0.05; ***p<0.001.
pi-2023-0027f1.jpg
Figure 2.
Three-way interaction effect of gaming time, loneliness, and living arrangement on PGU. The model included age as a covariate. *p<0.05; **p<0.01.
pi-2023-0027f2.jpg
Figure 3.
Conditional effects of gaming time on PGU depending on the level of loneliness based on living arrangements. The model included age as a covariate. The units for PGU are consistent with the original scale scores. Both the gaming time and loneliness scores have been standardized. *p<0.05; **p<0.01; ***p<0.001. SD, standard deviation.
pi-2023-0027f3.jpg
Table 1.
Demographic information (N=743)
Variable N (%)
Age (yr)
 18-29 279 (37.6)
 30-39 235 (31.6)
 40-49 150 (20.2)
 50-59 61 (8.2)
 60-69 17 (2.3)
 70-79 1 (0.1)
Employment
 Full time workers 504 (67.8)
 Temporary workers 100 (13.5)
 Unemployed/Students 139 (18.7)
Educational background
 Middle school graduate or lower 11 (1.5)
 High school graduate 280 (37.7)
 College graduate or higher 451 (60.7)
 No response 1 (0.1)
PGU
 High-risk group (>38.5) 194 (26.1)
 Low-risk group (≤38.5) 549 (73.9)
Living alone
 Living alone 148 (19.9)
 Living together 595 (80.1)

The high-risk group for PGU was classified based on the cutoff point (38.5) of the scale. PGU, problematic game use

Table 2.
Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations (N=743)
Variables Age Loneliness Living alone Household size Gaming time PGU
Age -
Loneliness 0.162*** -
Living alone 0.000 0.158*** -
Household size -0.066 -0.119** -0.742*** -
Gaming time -0.207*** 0.049 0.045 0.000 -
PGU -0.155*** 0.211*** 0.015 0.019 0.241*** -
Mean±SD 34.62±10.84 0.81±1.27 - 2.77±1.19 1.98±1.50 36.96±10.09

Loneliness, Loneliness and Social Isolation Scale; Living alone, 0 = living together, 1 = living alone; PGU, Game Overuse Screening Questionnaire (continuous).

** p<0.01;

*** p<0.001.

SD, standard deviation; PGU, problematic game use

Table 3.
Main, interaction, and conditional effects of gaming time and loneliness on PGU (N=743)
Main and interaction effects of gaming time and loneliness on PGU
Variable B SE t 95% CI
Independent variable
 Gaming time 1.268 0.240 5.285*** 0.797-1.739
 Loneliness 1.735 0.281 6.181*** 1.184-2.286
Two-way interaction
 Gaming time×loneliness 0.325 0.150 2.173* 0.031-0.619
Covariate
 Age -0.142 0.033 -4.271*** -0.208- -0.077
Conditional effect of gaming time on PGU by level of loneliness
Loneliness B SE t 95% CI
-1 SD 1.005 0.282 3.561*** 0.451-1.559
Mean 1.268 0.240 5.285*** 0.797-1.739
+1 SD 1.679 0.286 5.882*** 1.119-2.240

The estimates of the final model are presented.

* p<0.05;

*** p<0.001.

PGU, problematic gaming use; SE, standard error; CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation

Table 4.
Main, interaction, and conditional effects of gaming time, loneliness, and living alone on PGU (N=743)
Main and interaction effects of gaming time, loneliness, and living alone on PGU
Variables B SE t 95% CI
Independent variable
 Gaming time 0.805 0.258 3.114** 0.297-1.312
 Loneliness 2.199 0.328 6.714*** 1.556-2.842
 Living alone -0.671 0.884 -0.759 -2.407-1.064
Two-way interaction
 Gaming time×loneliness 0.143 0.165 0.870 -0.180-0.467
 Gaming time×living alone 2.718 0.622 4.368*** 1.497-3.940
 Loneliness×living alone -1.359 0.612 -2.220* -2.561--0.157
Three-way interaction
 Gaming time×loneliness×living alone 0.757 0.366 2.070* 0.039-1.475
Covariate
 Age -0.141 0.033 -4.269*** -0.206- -0.076
Conditional effect of gaming time×loneliness on PGU by living arrangements
Living arrangements B F df1 df2
Living alone 0.900 7.617** 1 734
Living together 0.143 0.756 1 734
Conditional effect of gaming time by loneliness in the living alone group
Loneliness B SE t 95% CI
-1 SD 2.796 0.666 4.200*** 1.489-4.103
Mean 3.523 0.568 6.206*** 2.408-4.637
+1 SD 4.663 0.642 7.264*** 3.402-5.923

The estimates of the final model are presented.

* p<0.05;

** p<0.01;

*** p<0.001.

PGU, problematic gaming use; SE, standard error; CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation

Table 5.
Main and interaction effects of gaming time, loneliness, and household size on PGU (N=595)
Variables B SE t 95% CI
Independent variable
 Gaming time 0.826 0.270 3.054** 0.294-1.355
 Loneliness 2.173 0.333 6.521*** 1.519-2.827
 Household size 0.313 0.437 0.717 -0.545-1.171
Two-way interaction
 Gaming time×loneliness 0.131 0.168 0.777 -0.200-0.461
 Gaming time×household size -0.142 0.323 -0.441 -0.776-0.491
 Loneliness×household size 0.333 0.382 0.872 -0.418-1.084
Three-way interaction
 Gaming time×loneliness×household size 0.152 0.251 0.606 -0.341-0.646
Covariate
 Age -0.136 0.037 -3.688*** -0.209- -0.064

The estimates of the final model are presented.

** p<0.01;

*** p<0.001.

PGU, problematic gaming use; SE, standard error; CI, confidence interval; SD, standard deviation

REFERENCES

1. Prato GD, Feijoo Gonzalez CA, Simon JP. Innovations in the video game industry: changing global markets. Commun Strateg 2014;94:17-38.

2. Number of video gamers worldwide in 2021. Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/293304/number-video-gamers. Accessed September 1, 2022.

3. ICT access and usage by households and individuals: ICT access and usage by individuals [Internet]. Available at: https://stats.oecd.org/Index.aspx?QueryId=110629. Accessed September 1, 2022.

4. A survey of game users 2021 [Internet]. Available at: https://www.kocca.kr/kocca/bbs/view/B0000147/1845578.do?menuNo=204153. Accessed September 1, 2022.

5. Wang HY, Cheng C. The associations between gaming motivation and internet gaming disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. JMIR Ment Health 2022;9:e23700
crossref pmid pmc
6. Griffiths MD, Király O, Pontes HM, Demetrovics Z. An overview of problematic gaming (1st ed). In: Aboujaoude E, Starcevic V, editor. Mental health in the digital age: grave dangers, great promise. New York: Oxford University Press, 2015, p. 27-45.

7. Boldi A, Rapp A, Tirassa M. Playing during a crisis: the impact of commercial video games on the reconfiguration of people’s life during the COVID-19 pandemic. Hum Comput Interact 2022 Mar 30 [Epub]. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370024.2022.2050725.
crossref pmid pmc
8. Vuorre M, Zendle D, Petrovskaya E, Ballou N, Przybylski AK. A large-scale study of changes to the quantity, quality, and distribution of video game play during a global health pandemic. Technol Mind Behav 2021;2:1-8.
crossref
9. Achab S, Nicolier M, Mauny F, Monnin J, Trojak B, Vandel P, et al. Massively multiplayer online role-playing games: comparing characteristics of addict vs non-addict online recruited gamers in a French adult population. BMC Psychiatry 2011;11:144
crossref pmid pmc pdf
10. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Internet gaming addiction: a systematic review of empirical research. Int J Ment Health Addict 2012;10:278-296.
crossref pdf
11. Ok C. Extraversion, loneliness, and problematic game use: a longitudinal study. Pers Individ Differ 2021;168:110290
crossref
12. ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics (ICD-11 MMS) [Internet]. Available at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#. Accessed September 1, 2022.

13. Stevens MW, Dorstyn D, Delfabbro PH, King DL. Global prevalence of gaming disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2021;55:553-568.
crossref pmid pdf
14. Ma SS, Worhunsky PD, Xu JS, Yip SW, Zhou N, Zhang JT, et al. Alterations in functional networks during cue-reactivity in internet gaming disorder. J Behav Addict 2019;8:277-287.
crossref pmid pmc
15. Kim HS, Son G, Roh EB, Ahn WY, Kim J, Shin SH, et al. Prevalence of gaming disorder: a meta-analysis. Addict Behav 2022;126:107183
crossref pmid
16. Cudo A, Dobosz M, Griffiths MD, Kuss DJ. The relationship between early maladaptive schemas, depression, anxiety and problematic video gaming among female and male gamers. Int J Ment Health Addict 2022 Jun 27 [Epub]. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-022-00858-2.
crossref
17. Cudo A, Wojtasiński M, Tużnik P, Fudali-Czyż A, Griffiths MD. The relationship between depressive symptoms, loneliness, self-control, and gaming disorder among Polish male and female gamers: the indirect effects of gaming motives. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022;19:10438
crossref pmid pmc
18. Biolcati R, Passini S, Griffiths MD. All-in and bad beat: professional poker players and pathological gambling. Int J Ment Health Addict 2015;13:19-32.
crossref pdf
19. Bőthe B, Tóth-Király I, Potenza MN, Orosz G, Demetrovics Z. High-frequency pornography use may not always be problematic. J Sex Med 2020;17:793-811.
crossref pmid pdf
20. Brunborg GS, Mentzoni RA, Frøyland LR. Is video gaming, or video game addiction, associated with depression, academic achievement, heavy episodic drinking, or conduct problems? J Behav Addict 2014;3:27-32.
crossref pmid pmc
21. Király O, Tóth D, Urbán R, Demetrovics Z, Maraz A. Intense video gaming is not essentially problematic. Psychol Addict Behav 2017;31:807-817.
crossref pmid
22. Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Gentile DA. The internet gaming disorder scale. Psychol Assess 2015;27:567-582.
crossref pmid
23. Shu MY, Ivan Jacob AP, Meng Xuan Z, Anise MSW. Psychometric validation of the internet gaming disorder-20 test among Chinese middle school and university students. J Behav Addict 2019;8:295-305.
crossref pmid pmc
24. Choi J, Cho H, Lee S, Kim J, Park EC. Effect of the online game shutdown policy on internet use, internet addiction, and sleeping hours in Korean adolescents. J Adolesc Health 2018;62:548-555.
crossref pmid
25. Zhan JD, Chan HC. Government regulation of online game addiction. Commun Assoc Inf Syst 2012;30:187-198.
crossref
26. Hwang SJ, Hong JP, An JH, Kim MH, Jeong SH, Chang H. Development and validation of loneliness and social isolation scale. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc 2021;60:291-297.
crossref pdf
27. Chen C, Leung L. Are you addicted to Candy Crush Saga? An exploratory study linking psychological factors to mobile social game addiction. Telemat Inform 2016;33:1155-1166.
crossref
28. Nebel S, Ninaus M. Does playing apart really bring us together? Investigating the link between perceived loneliness and the use of video games during a period of social distancing. Front Psychol 2022;13:683842
crossref pmid pmc
29. Wang JL, Sheng JR, Wang HZ. The association between mobile game addiction and depression, social anxiety, and loneliness. Front Public Health 2019;7:247
crossref pmid pmc
30. Stockdale L, Coyne SM. Video game addiction in emerging adulthood: cross-sectional evidence of pathology in video game addicts as compared to matched healthy controls. J Affect Disord 2018;225:265-272.
crossref pmid
31. Lieberz J, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Saporta N, Esser T, Kuskova E, Stoffel-Wagner B, et al. Loneliness and the social brain: how perceived social isolation impairs human interactions. Adv Sci (Weinh) 2021;8:e2102076
crossref pmid
32. Grant JE, Chamberlain SR, Schreiber LR, Odlaug BL. Gender-related clinical and neurocognitive differences in individuals seeking treatment for pathological gambling. J Psychiatr Res 2012;46:1206-1211.
crossref pmid pmc
33. Xu XP, Liu QQ, Li ZH, Yang WX. The mediating role of loneliness and the moderating role of gender between peer phubbing and adolescent mobile social media addiction. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022;19:10176
crossref pmid pmc
34. Vermeulen L, Van Looy J, De Grove F, Courtois C. >You are what you play?: a quantitative study into game design preferences across gender and their interaction with gaming habits. Proceedings of DiGRA 2011 conference: think design play; 2011 Sep 14-17; Hilversum, Netherlands. Tampere: DiGRA; 2011.

35. Terlecki M, Brown J, Harner-Steciw L, Irvin-Hannum J, Marchetto-Ryan N, Ruhl L, et al. Sex differences and similarities in video game experience, preferences, and self-efficacy: implications for the gaming industry. Curr Psychol 2011;30:22-33.
crossref pdf
36. Gierveld Jde J, van Groenou MB, Hoogendoorn AW, Smit JH. Quality of marriages in later life and emotional and social loneliness. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2009;64:497-506.
pmid
37. Victor CR, Yang K. The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J Psychol 2012;146:85-104.
crossref pmid
38. Fujii R, Konno Y, Tateishi S, Hino A, Tsuji M, Ikegami K, et al. Association between time spent with family and loneliness among Japanese workers during the COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional study. Front Psychiatry 2021;12:786400
crossref pmid pmc
39. Gjoneska B, Potenza MN, Jones J, Corazza O, Hall N, Sales CMD, et al. Problematic use of the internet during the COVID-19 pandemic: good practices and mental health recommendations. Compr Psychiatry 2022;112:152279
crossref pmid pmc
40. Kim JM, Lee E. Association between healthy eating index and mental health in middle-aged adults based on household size in Korea. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2022;19:4692
crossref pmid pmc
41. Loton D, Borkoles E, Lubman D, Polman R. Video game addiction, engagement and symptoms of stress, depression and anxiety: the mediating role of coping. Int J Ment Health Addiction 2016;14:565-578.
crossref pdf
42. Sunwoo L. Loneliness among older adults in the Czech Republic: a socio-demographic, health, and psychosocial profile. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2020;90:104068
crossref pmid
43. Bland AR, Roiser JP, Mehta MA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Elliott R. The impact of COVID-19 social isolation on aspects of emotional and social cognition. Cogn Emot 2022;36:49-58.
crossref pmid
44. Oh J, Neal ZP. Two’s company, but four’s a crowd: the relationship among COVID-19 stress, household size, and life satisfaction. Collabra Psychol 2021;7:24923
crossref pdf
45. Okabe-Miyamoto K, Folk D, Lyubomirsky S, Dunn EW. Changes in social connection during COVID-19 social distancing: it’s not (household) size that matters, it’s who you’re with. PLoS One 2021;16:e0245009
crossref pmid pmc
46. Baek IC, Kim JH, Joung YS, Lee HW, Park S, Park EJ, et al. Development and validation study of game overuse screening questionnaire. Psychiatry Res 2020;290:113165
crossref pmid
47. Iacobucci D, Schneider MJ, Popovich DL, Bakamitsos GA. Mean centering helps alleviate “micro” but not “macro” multicollinearity. Behav Res Methods 2016;48:1308-1317.
crossref pmid pdf
48. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach (2nd ed). New York: Guilford Publications; 2018.

49. Oka T, Hamamura T, Miyake Y, Kobayashi N, Honjo M, Kawato M, et al. Prevalence and risk factors of internet gaming disorder and problematic internet use before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: a large online survey of Japanese adults. J Psychiatr Res 2021;142:218-225.
crossref pmid
50. Wittek CT, Finserås TR, Pallesen S, Mentzoni RA, Hanss D, Griffiths MD, et al. Prevalence and predictors of video game addiction: a study based on a national representative sample of gamers. Int J Ment Health Addict 2016;14:672-686.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
51. Park K, Chang H, Kim K, Cho YI. The variety of gaming devices and game over-dependence among middle school students: a moderated mediation model of time spent and gender. Kor J Psychol Health 2022;27:251-270.

52. Wölfling K, Müller KW, Dreier M, Ruckes C, Deuster O, Batra A, et al. Efficacy of short-term treatment of internet and computer game addiction: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 2019;76:1018-1025.
crossref pmid pmc
53. Gasteiger N, Loveys K, Law M, Broadbent E. Friends from the future: a scoping review of research into robots and computer agents to combat loneliness in older people. Clin Interv Aging 2021;16:941-971.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
54. Shapira S, Yeshua-Katz D, Cohn-Schwartz E, Aharonson-Daniel L, Sarid O, Clarfield AM. A pilot randomized controlled trial of a group intervention via Zoom to relieve loneliness and depressive symptoms among older persons during the COVID-19 outbreak. Internet Interv 2021;24:100368
crossref pmid pmc
TOOLS
Share:
Facebook Twitter Linked In Google+
METRICS Graph View
  • 0 Crossref
  •   Scopus
  • 1,797 View
  • 81 Download


ABOUT
AUTHOR INFORMATION
ARTICLE CATEGORY

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
Editorial Office
#522, 27, Seochojungang-ro 24-gil, Seocho-gu, Seoul 06601, Korea
Tel: +82-2-717-0892    E-mail: psychiatryinvest@gmail.com                

Copyright © 2024 by Korean Neuropsychiatric Association.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next