Psychiatry Investig Search

CLOSE


Psychiatry Investig > Volume 20(8); 2023 > Article
Qin, Wang, Li, and Zhang: Selective Attentional Networks Deficits in Parents of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder: Evidence From Attention Network Test

Abstract

Objective

We primarily aimed to investigate the attention network function among parents of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) using the Attention Network Test (ANT). The secondary objective was to observe whether the three attention networks of all participants were related to each other.

Methods

We included 28 parents of children with ASD and 28 well-matched parents of typically developing children. All participants underwent the neuropsychological assessment and ANT test. The three distinct attention networks, including alerting, orienting, and executive control, were also measured.

Results

Compared with controls, parents of children with ASD showed less-efficient alerting and executive control network (all p<0.05), but not orienting network (p=0.74). No significant correlation was found between the alerting, orienting, and executive control network for either group.

Conclusion

Our findings showed that parents of children with ASD had deficits in alerting and executive control attention functions. The deficits are indications of a broad autism phenotype.

INTRODUCTION

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a lifelong neurodevelopmental condition that affects social, communicative, behavioral, and cognitive functioning [1,2]. The prevalence rates of autism have increased dramatically over the past decades, and the new estimate is a global prevalence of 1% [3].
Individuals with ASD typically exhibit a common characteristic, that is, some form of attention deficit, although they differ in age and range of abilities [4]. Studies have documented that individuals with ASD, including children with ASD [5], show deficits when performing tasks involving attention [6-8]. Attentional deficits in ASD are commonly observed in social contexts, however, it occasionally occurs in elementary forms of attentional processing such as simple target detection tasks as well as tests of vigilance or sustained attention [7]. Neuroimaging research has revealed different patterns of brain activity in children with ASD, even when ASD children and neurotypical children show the same performance on attention tasks [9]. Additionally, a meta-analysis by van der Meer et al. [10] showed that children with ASD had deficits in both visual and auditory attention, indicating that the deficiency was not modality specific. Specifically, individuals with ASD show increased activation in the frontal lobe, which is thought to reflect the recruitment of alternative brain regions as a means of compensating for achieving the same level of attentional performance [9]. Although individuals with ASD often exhibit attentional difficulties, the exact nature of their attentional deficits remains unclear.
As a vital cognitive function, human attention work is a complex physiological process. It integrates external sensory inputs with internal information and decision-making and requires the interaction of many subcomponents to complete even the simplest attention task [11,12]. Now that attentional dysfunction have been demonstrated to be a consistent observation in ASD. It would be worthwhile to isolate the subcomponents of attention to determine whether these impairments represent a global deficits or localized dysfunction [13]. Understanding the underlying locus of the attentional deficits as exhibited by individuals with ASD would contribute to managing and treating their attentional problems.
The attention network theory proposed by Posner and Petersen [11] provides a useful theoretical framework for identifying the subcomponents of attention. This theory suggests that the attention system can be divided into three distinct subcomponents, that is, alerting, orienting, and executive control. These three neural networks have been shown to be functionally and structurally independent [14] and are related to specific neural substrates and specific functions [11,12,15,16]. Specifically, alerting specializes in the achievement and maintenance of appropriate sensitivity to incoming stimuli and is associated with activation in the locus coeruleus, right frontal and parietal brain areas. Orienting is responsible for selecting information from numerous sensory inputs, and is associated with the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), superior parietal lobe, and frontal eye fields (FEFs). Executive control involves the resolution of response conflicts among competing sources of input and is related to the anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal cortex. But they may interact to influence performance [17,18].
Based on attention network theory [11], Fan et al. [14] developed the Attention Network Test (ANT) to simultaneously evaluate the three aforementioned attention components within one experiment. Participants were instructed to determine the direction of the “target” arrow by pressing a key as quickly as possible [14]. The efficiency value of each network is determined by calculating the average response time under several different prompts and flanker conditions [19]. To date, the ANT has been widely utilized to assess the attention function in various psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia,20 depression [21], attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [22], as well as ASD [8].
Previous studies used the ANT task to investigate the attention in ASD, showing different results with deficits being observed in three attention components. A study done by Fan et al. [23] found weaker alerting attention and executive attention in 20 adults with ASD compared to healthy controls, while no differences were found for orienting attention. Another two study in children with ASD showed different result [8,24]. One was found that, compared with the control group, orienting and executive control attention was weaker, but the other study found that the attention deficit in children with ASD was only existed in the orienting attention.
Genetic factors have been considered to play a significant role in the pathophysiological mechanisms of ASD. Twin studies and epidemiological studies indicated that the heritability of ASD is estimated to be approximately 80%-90% [25,26]. This means that ASD is a highly genetically neuropsychiatric disorder [27]. A considerable number of family members of children with ASD, especially in first-degree relatives (including parents, siblings, and offspring), were found to exhibit a set of characteristics that are resemble some of the core symptoms of ASD [28,29]. These characteristics are inadequate to meet the diagnostic criteria for ASD, and they are termed as a broad autism phenotype (BAP) [30,31]. However, thus far, few studies have focused on the attentional capacities of first-degree relatives of individuals with ASD. It remains unclear as to whether first-degree relatives with ASD share similar attentional dysfunction as those observed in ASD.
This study aimed to investigate and compare the differences in attention function between parents of children with ASD and parents of typical developmental children (TDC). We specifically designed to employ the ANT task to simultaneously investigate the three subcomponents of attention system in each group. According to the literature, we predicted that parents of children with ASD would exhibit less efficient alerting, orienting, and executive attention, compared to controls, or show selective impairment of attention subcomponents.

METHODS

We recruited 28 parents of children with ASD (9 men, 19 women) from autism rehabilitation centers. According to the diagnostic criteria for ASD, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition [32], a structured clinical interview was used to confirm that these recruited parents were not diagnosed with ASD by experienced clinical psychiatrists, and excluded with other neurodevelopmental disorders and other psychiatric disorders. Furthermore, their children were diagnosed with ASD by more than two child psychiatrists. Twenty-eight healthy controls (8 men, 20 women), enrolled from a primary school, were matched with parents of children with ASD for age, gender, and educational level. Two experienced psychiatrists assessed the healthy controls and their children, and confirmed that they all did not meet the diagnostic criteria for ASD.
None of the participants had a history of neuropsychiatric disorders, head injury, or substance abuse. And they were all right-handed. All participants gave their written informed consent prior to the experiment according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol of this study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Bengbu Medical College (IRB No. 2019-061).

Measures

Neuropsychological evaluation

All participants underwent neuropsychological tests to evaluate their basic cognitive conditions. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) test was used to measure general cognitive function [33]. The Digit Span (DS) (forward, backward) of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale was used to assess attention [34]. Trail Making Test (TMT) (A, B) was used to evaluate common execution functions [35]. We used the Chinese version Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) to evaluate the autistic characteristics and broader autism phenotypes of all participants [36]. On average, it took 25-30 minutes for participants to complete the assessment. Skilled psychologists and psychiatrists administered all assessments.

ANT

The ANT task required the participants to watch the stimuli presented on the computer screen, and we collected the reaction time (RT) by two corresponding keys. The stimuli consisted of five horizontal arrows that pointed to the left or right on a gray background. The target item was presented in the center of the five arrows. As an arrow faced left or right, the arrows on both sides of the target created three possible conditions: neutral condition, congruent condition, and incongruent condition. When appeared neutral condition, the arrows on both sides do not appear. In the congruent condition, the arrows on both sides pointed in the same direction as the target item; when appeared incongruent condition, the directions of the arrows on both sides were inconsistent with the target item. Throughout the task, participants were required to focus on the centrally located fixation cross, and they needed to press the keyboard direction keys as fast and accurately as possible by pressing the corresponding button with their finger of their left or right hand. Each stimulus of the test was divided into four steps, and then repeated cycles until the end of the task. In the first step, a fixation point “+” appeared in the screen randomly, and this point would last for 400-1,600 ms. In the second step, the warning cue would or would not appear for a fixed time of 100 ms. In the third step, a fixation point with a duration of 400 ms appeared on the screen. In the last step, the target stimulus appeared until the participants responded by pressing the button. In this step, the stimuli could last up to 2,700 ms. If the participants completed the response shorter than 2,700 ms, the target stimulus would disappear. The computer would adjust the duration based on the duration and RT of the first step after the participants pressed the button of the target stimulus to ensure a fixed experimental interval.
There were four different cue conditions: no cue, central cue, double cue, and spatial cue. In the no cue condition, the fixation cross appeared directly in the center of the screen for only 100 ms. In the central cue condition, the cue was located at the center of the fixation point. In the double cue condition, cue position appeared above and below the fixation point, this kind of cue does not provide any spatial information and is just a hint of the stimulus to come for participants. In the spatial cue condition, the cue position appeared directly above or below the fixation point, this kind of cue reminded the participants of the target location.
Each ANT task consisted of four blocks. The first block consisted of 24 full feedback exercises. The subsequent three blocks consisted of 96 trials which included 48 different conditions: 4 types of cues×3 flanker conditions×2 target locations×2 target stimuli directions, with two repetitions, and the order of the trails presented to the participants was random. The experiment was carried out in a quiet environment. Figure 1 illustrates the ANT task.

Calculation of the three subtypes of attention networks

We calculated the efficiencies of the alerting, orienting, and executive control networks using the RTs under different test conditions, including 12 conditions as described above. Firstly, the mean RT of the no cue condition minus the mean RT of the double cue condition was used to represent the efficiency of the alerting network. The larger the score of the alerting network efficiency, the higher the alerting attention function of the participants. Secondly, the mean RT in the central cue condition minus the mean RT in the spatial cue condition is used to represent the efficiency of the orienting network. The larger the value of orienting network efficiency, the higher the orienting attention function of the participants. Lastly, we calculated the efficiency of the executive control network by using the mean RT of the congruent target conditions minus the RT of the incongruent target conditions [14]. The greater the value of the executive control attention network efficiency, the lower the executive control function of the participants.

Statistical analyses

We used SPSS, version 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) for Windows to examine all statistical analyses. According to this study, a p<0.05 (two-tail) was considered to be statistically significant. We used independent sample t-test to analyze the neuropsychological data (including age, education, DS, TMT, MoCA, and AQ), background information data, and three efficiency values of attention networks of the two groups. And we used the chi-square test to compare the gender differences between the two groups. Apart from these, we applied Pearson’s correlation test to assess the relationships between three networks’ efficiency and the relationships between the AQ and the three networks’ efficiencies.

RESULTS

Neuropsychological and background tests

Table 1 shows the detailed demographic statistics and neuropsychological data of the participants. Independent sample t-tests, chi-square test showed no difference between parents of children with ASD and parents of TDC in age, education, gender, DS, TMT, MoCA, and AQ (p>0.05).

Efficiencies of the three subtypes of attention networks

The results of participants’ ANT are shown in Table 2. The data shows that the mean RT of alerting network of parents of children with ASD was significantly shorter than that of the parents of TDC (p=0.007). While the mean RT of the executive control network of parents of children with ASD was significantly longer than that of the parents of TDC (p=0.011). But there were no significant differences between the two groups in the orienting network (p=0.740). In addition, there were no significant statistical differences between the two groups in the overall mean RT and global accuracy.
Figure 2 shows the scores of three networks ratios between parents of children with ASD and parents of TDC. There were significant differences in the alerting and executive control networks (p=0.009, p=0.016, respectively), but there was no significant difference in the orienting network (p=0.77). It indicated that there were selective differences in the alerting and executive control networks, but the orienting network was not affected.

Correlations between attention networks and AQ

In order to examine the relative independence of three subtypes of attention networks in the literature, we used Pearson correlation analysis to analyze the data of 56 participants. The results shows that there was no significant correlation between alerting, orienting, and executive in any combination of the two groups. Table 3 illustrates the results of the calculations.
As for the relationship between the three subtypes of attention networks and various dimensions of AQ. The results shows that there was no significant correlation between the scores of the three attention networks and the various dimensions of AQ, whether it is the parents of children with ASD or the parents of children with TDC. Table 4 illustrates the results of the calculations.

DISCUSSION

This study aimed to use ANT, a nonverbal, nonsocial task, to measure whether there are differences in attention function between parents of children with ASD and parents of TDC. The results showed that there were significant differences in alerting and executive control networks between parents of children with ASD and parents of TDC, which showed that parents of children with ASD had decreased alerting and executive control functions. Moreover, we found no significant functional correlation between the alerting, orienting, and executive control functions of the attention network, which showed that these three attention subtypes were independent of each other. As far as we know, this study was one of the few studies that used the ANT task to analyze the attention function of parents of children with ASD. Our findings suggested that selective impairment of attentional function in parents of children with ASD was an indication of a BAP. Our study showed that, with comparable correctness rates, the alerting network efficiency of the parents of children with ASD was significantly lower than that of TDC parents. This finding indicated that parents of children with ASD had a weaker alerting function than parents of TDC, and was unable to react faster to what happened in their environment. According to the alerting network system, the alerting network system is a state in which the body is able to react quickly to its surroundings in order to receive incoming information more quickly, and it is mainly responsible for maintaining the sensitive state of receiving incoming information. Evidence from previous studies showed that children with autism were less sensitive to new things [37,38]. And it has been reported in the literature that autistic traits negatively regulate normal people’s perception of social stimulation, such as emotional faces [39]. These results indicated that autism or autistic traits would affect the alerting function of normal individuals to some extent. Therefore, in order to figure out the mechanism of impaired alerting network function and genetic characteristics of parents of children with ASD, the specific neural mechanism of parents of children with ASD needs to be further researched.
Our study results demonstrated that the efficiency of the executive control network of the parents of children with ASD was significantly longer than those of the parents of TDC. This indicated that when dealing with incongruent flanker information, it was difficult for the parents of children with ASD to ignore the conflicting directional information represented by the incongruent flanker information. In order to obtain higher accuracy, it took more time to deal with conflicting directional information. According to the result of McDowell et al. [40] and Petersen and Posner [41] on the neural aspects of the executive control network, the executive control network was regulated by two different neural circuitries: 1) Cinguo-opericular control system, which performed the functions of attention behavior monitoring and task-set maintenance, and the function of stable maintenance throughout the whole control process. 2) The frontoparietal control system, which performed the functions of task-set initiation and real-time adjustment. Previous studies have shown that individuals with ASD have diminished activity levels of anterior cingulate compared with normal adults [23]. With regard to the mechanism of this defect, we hypothesized that this may account for the poorer executive attentional function of the parents of children with ASD compared with the parents of TDC in the present experiment.
Notably, we found that there is no significant difference in orienting attention efficiency between parents of children with ASD and parents of TDC. However, there are many factors that affect the orienting efficiency. We still could not tell whether the parents of children with ASD had abnormalities in the orienting attention network. First of all, similar to executive control attention, Petersen and Posner [41] proposed the existence of two types of neural circuits governing the orienting attention: 1) Dorsal network system: this system works primarily on fast strategic control of attention, and it is composed of FEFs and the intraparietal sulcus. 2) Ventral network system: this system is more active in tracking targets, and it is considered to be a part of the response network for sensing events. It consists of the TPJ and the ventral frontal cortex [41]. These two attention systems jointly determine the orienting attention network. Based on the existing research results on ASD, there are inconsistent researches between two systems of orienting attention network [42]. Renner et al. [43] reported that the deficits in the orienting attention network of ASD only existed in the ventral system, while an intact dorsal system is preserved [43]. And a subset of studies found that the dorsal system in ASD was enhanced [44]. In addition, to some extent, the age of the participants also affects the orienting network efficiency values. Based on the results of the existing literature, we found that the dorsal system visuospatial function is lower and plastic in children compared to adults. But ANT has shortcomings: it cannot distinguish whether the abnormally orienting attention network present in the participants was a dorsal system or ventral system. Therefore, we have the following three speculations: 1) Parents of children with ASD retain intact orienting networks. 2) Dorsal system and ventral system in parents of children with ASD are not synchronized in efficiency, and there may be an enhancement of the dorsal system [44] along with an impairment of the ventral system brain areas [43], but there is some extent of neutralization in terms of overall effect. And ANT can only make judgments in general and cannot be specific to the two attention systems of orienting [3]. 3) The dorsal system has a certain plasticity. The participants in this experiment were all parents of children with ASD, with an average age of 34 years. The dorsal system of parents of children with ASD was enhanced with age, and their original differences in the orienting network gradually disappeared.
In addition, our results showed that parents of children with ASD had selective impairment of attention function, similar to the results of Hughes et al.’s [45] research on the executive control network of parents of children with ASD. Hughes et al.’s [45] findings showed that parents of autistic children had impairment of executive control function. This impairment was only observed in a quarter of parents of children with ASD, but its incidence was 5-10 times higher than that of learning disability and control group. This indicated that poor attentional function was a characteristic of parents of children with ASD, rather than a universal phenomenon. In our study, it is worth noting that the impairment not only lies in the executive control function, but also in the alerting function. We suggest that it was due to a broader autistic phenotype, or BAP, resulting from a higher-than-normal proportion of autism-related impairments in relatives of autistic individuals due to genetic factors. But it is a challenging task to define the boundaries of BAP. In previous work, family history interviews have been used to determine that the proband of autism has social and communication impairments, but there is no effective method to specifically diagnose BAP. Some people believed that executive function deficiency and other cognitive defects would jointly produce corresponding diseases, and the generation of ASD is related to the interaction of executive function defects and facial processing defects. Hill [46] found that the impairment of executive function might be one of the promising candidate factors. Hughes et al.[47] believed that to determine the genetic tendency of ASD, it is necessary to investigate which component of executive function is the dominant factor. In addition, a study by Wong et al. [48] showed that parents of children with ASD were worse than normal people in the two executive functions of ideational fluency and generative ability, and fathers of children with ASD also had defects in setting shifting to a previously unrelated dimension. Our experimental results are consistent with those of Wong et al. [48]. The genetic tendency of ASD is more complex, and the study of its inner phenotype is more helpful for the specific diagnosis of BAP. The selective impairment of executive function (such as conceptual fluency and generative ability) can be used as an auxiliary diagnostic criterion in the later confirmation of the BAP, and the core defects of executive function in ASD can be further comprehensively understood in future research. This will be more conducive to the diagnosis and treatment of ASD.
In addition, we noticed that no significant relationship was discovered between the three attention networks in the two groups. This finding implies that three subtypes of networks of participants were functionally independent and did not interfere with each other among ASD parents and TDC parents. Similarly, a study conducted by Fan et al. [14] reported that the three subtypes of attention networks were independent of each other in ANT tests of normal adults. However, Mutreja et al. [8] found that there was a significant relationship between the alerting and orienting network in both children with ASD and neurotypical children, but no significant correlation was found between orienting and alerting with executive control network in either group. In the future, maybe we should add some larger-scale in-depth experiments and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) images to verify the correlations between the three subtypes of attention networks in the first-degree relatives of ASD.
Because of practical constraints, four limitations of our study should be presented. First, the sample size included in our experiments was small and participants were selected mainly included mothers, not both parents. ASD shows different psychological and phenotypic characteristics in girls, and the genetic transmission path may be different from boys. Second, although ANT can be well observed by dividing the human attentional function into three parts, but it can be further subdivided according to the neural circuitry that regulates it. ANT cannot make further observations. Thirdly, we only divided the participants into the ASD group and the control group, but did not divide the ASD group into two or more groups according to the symptom severity of ASD and analysis of variance between groups. Finally, while the ANT task can give intuitive values of the efficiency of the relevant attention system, however, attentional function involves the activation or inhibition of relevant brain regions, which is a complex and unpredictable process. No imageological examinations were performed in this study. In future research works, larger samples may be used to verify our findings. If conditions permit, participants should have a functional imageological examination (e.g., functional MRI). This can help us to study the formation mechanism of attention deficit of parents of children with ASD more accurately.
In conclusion, the results of our study showed that the attentional function of parents of children with ASD was selectively impaired in the alerting and executive control network. The efficiency of the executive control network was higher than that of parents of TDC, indicated that the executive control function was weakened. In the meanwhile, the efficiency of the alerting network was obviously lower, which indicated that the alerting function of parents of children with ASD was decreased too. There was no significant correlation between the three networks of parents of children with ASD and parents of TDC, and they were three functionally independent attention systems. Moreover, the correlation analysis between AQ and the three attention networks of the two groups showed that there was no significant relationship, suggesting that we cannot predict the efficiency of attention networks through AQ scores. Our results suggested that autism, a genetic factor disorder of neurocognitive dysplasia, and abnormalities in the alerting and executive control network of parents of children with ASD are part of BAP. Our results represented a further increase in our understanding of attention deficits in parents of children with ASD and expand the understanding of selected attentional deficits in parents of children with ASD.

Notes

Availability of Data and Material

The datasets generated or analyzed during the study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Shunhua Zhang. Data curation: Tian Qin, Shunhua Zhang. Formal analysis: all authors. Funding acquisition: Shunhua Zhang. Investigation: all authors. Methodology: Tian Qin, Shunhua Zhang. Project administration: Shunhua Zhang. Resources: Shunhua Zhang. Software: all authors. Supervision: Shunhua Zhang. Validation: Tian Qin, Shunhua Zhang. Visualization: Tian Qin, Shunhua Zhang. Writing—original draff: Tian Qin. Writing—review & editing: Shunhua Zhang.

Funding Statement

This work was funded by the National Innovation and Entrepreneurship Training Program for college students of China under Grant 201910367009, 202010367036, 202110367049, and 202210367022, as well as Anhui Provincial Colleges and Universities Ideological and Political Work Ability Improvement Project (No. sztsjh2019-7-14), and the Key Projects of Teaching Research for Colleges and Universities in Anhui Province, China (No. 2020jyxm1174).

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We would like to appreciate all the participants for their unique contribution. We are also grateful for the valuable support from Anhui Province Key Laboratory of Cognition and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, and Gongjun Ji, Dandan Li, and Tingting Yang.

Figure 1.
The protocol scheme for the ANT task. A: The four cue conditions. B: The six stimuli used in the present task. C: An example of the procedure. ANT, Attention Network Test.
pi-2022-0248f1.jpg
Figure 2.
Network ratio scores of parents of children with ASD and parents of TDC. *p<0.05; **p<0.01. ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typical developmental children.
pi-2022-0248f2.jpg
Table 1.
Demographic and neuropsychological performances of ASD parents and TDC parents
Measure ASD parents (N=28) TDC parents (N=28) t or χ2 p
Demographic data
Age (yr) 34.43±6.14 34.11±3.88 0.234* 0.816
Gender, man/woman 9/19 8/20 0.084 0.771
Education (yr) 12.36±2.95 13.21±3.46 -0.998* 0.323
Neuropsychological performances
DS-Forward 8.04±0.10 8.42±1.14 -1.374* 0.175
DS-Backward 5.50±0.92 5.89±1.40 -1.242* 0.220
TMT-A (sec) 44.22±12.82 39.77±14.35 1.227* 0.225
TMT-B (sec) 85.13±25.14 74.86±18.03 1.758* 0.084
MoCA 26.54±1.90 27.00±1.36 -1.053* 0.297
Total AQ 111.98±12.15 110.61±9.55 0.469* 0.641
Social skill 22.93±4.58 21.71±3.81 1.079* 0.285
Attention switching 25.07±2.65 24.71±3.48 0.432* 0.668
Attention to detail 22.44±4.74 23.00±3.92 -0.483* 0.631
Communication 20.85±4.86 20.36±4.53 0.398* 0.692
Imagination 20.68±2.38 20.82±2.93 -0.195* 0.846

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation or number.

* independent sample t-tests;

χ2 test.

There are no statistical differences between the two groups in demographic data and neuropsychological performances (all p>0.05).

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typical developmental children; DS, The Digital Span Test; TMT, Trail Making Test; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; AQ, Autism Spectrum Quotient

Table 2.
Attention network scores and accuracy of ASD parents and TDC parents
ASD parents TDC parents t p
Alerting (ms) 26.00±22.31 41.79±19.77 2.80 0.007**
Orienting (ms) 36.39±16.46 34.54±24.37 -0.33 0.740
Executive control (ms) 121.79±32.84 98.89±31.97 -2.64 0.011*
Overall mean RT (ms) 646.21±90.40 653.25±73.59 1.03 0.751
Accuracy (%) 98.82±1.28 99.11±0.74 0.32 0.310

Values are presented as mean RT±standard deviation.

* represents a significantly higher mean RT in the ASD group than in the TDC group (p<0.05);

** represents a significantly lower mean RT in the ASD group than in the TDC group (p<0.01).

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typical developmental children; RT, reaction time

Table 3.
Correlation analysis between attentional networks
ASD parents
TDC parents
Alerting Orienting Executive Alerting Orienting Executive
Alerting 1 1
Orienting 0.274 1 0.148 1
Executive 0.179 0.005 1 -0.001 -0.089 1

There are no statistical correlations between the three attentional subsystems for both groups (p>0.05). ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typically developing children

Table 4.
Correlation analysis between attentional networks and AQ
ASD parents
TDC parents
Alerting Orienting Executive Alerting Orienting Executive
AQ-total 0.096 -0.190 0.036 0.096 -0.190 0.036
AQ-S 0.184 -0.077 0.104 0.184 -0.077 0.104
AQ-AS -0.09 -0.259 0.010 -0.009 -0.259 0.010
AQ-AD 0.227 0.059 -0.239 0.227 0.059 -0.239
AQ-C -0.133 -0.338 0.209 -0.133 -0.338 0.209
AQ-IM -0.012 0.076 -0.023 -0.012 0.076 -0.023

There are no statistical correlations between the three subtypes of attention network and various dimensions of AQ for both groups (p>0.05). ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typical developmental children; AQ, Autism Spectrum Quotient; AQ-total, total AQ; AQ-S, AQ-social skill; AQ-AS, AQ-attention switch; AQ-AD, AQ-attention to detail; AQ-C, AQ-communication; AQ-IM, AQ-imagination

REFERENCES

1. Lyall K, Croen L, Daniels J, Fallin MD, Ladd-Acosta C, Lee BK, et al. The changing epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annu Rev Public Health 2017;38:81-102.
crossref pmid pmc
2. Baker KG. Evaluation of DSM-5 and IWG-2 criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Diagnosis (Berl) 2016;3:9-12.
crossref pmid
3. Zeidan J, Fombonne E, Scorah J, Ibrahim A, Durkin MS, Saxena S, et al. Global prevalence of autism: a systematic review update. Autism Res 2022;15:778-790.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
4. Matson JL, Rieske RD, Williams LW. The relationship between autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an overview. Res Dev Disabil 2013;34:2475-2484.
crossref pmid
5. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Learning, attention, writing, and processing speed in typical children and children with ADHD, autism, anxiety, depression, and oppositional-defiant disorder. Child Neuropsychol 2007;13:469-493.
crossref pmid
6. Corbett BA, Constantine LJ. Autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: assessing attention and response control with the integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test. Child Neuropsychol 2006;12:335-348.
crossref pmid
7. Teder-Sälejärvi WA, Pierce KL, Courchesne E, Hillyard SA. Auditory spatial localization and attention deficits in autistic adults. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2005;23:221-234.
crossref pmid
8. Mutreja R, Craig C, O’Boyle MW. Attentional network deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev Neurorehabil 2016;19:389-397.
crossref pmid
9. Vaidya CJ, Foss-Feig J, Shook D, Kaplan L, Kenworthy L, Gaillard WD. Controlling attention to gaze and arrows in childhood: an fMRI study of typical development and autism spectrum disorders. Dev Sci 2011;14:911-924.
crossref pmid
10. van der Meer JM, Oerlemans AM, van Steijn DJ, Lappenschaar MG, de Sonneville LM, Buitelaar JK, et al. Are autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder different manifestations of one overarching disorder? Cognitive and symptom evidence from a clinical and population-based sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012;51:1160-1172.e3.
crossref pmid
11. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 1990;13:25-42.
crossref pmid
12. Posner MI, Fan J. Attention as an organ system. In: Pomerantz JR, editor. Topics in integrative neuroscience: from cells to cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2008, p. 31-61.

13. Hames EC, Rajmohan R, Fang D, Anderson R, Baker M, Richman DM, et al. Attentional networks in adolescents with high-functioning autism: an fMRI investigation. Open Neuroimag J 2016;10:102-110.
crossref pmid pmc
14. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:340-347.
crossref pmid pdf
15. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annu Rev Psychol 2007;58:1-23.
crossref pmid
16. Posner MI, Rothbart MK. Attention, self-regulation and consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1998;353:1915-1927.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
17. Callejas A, Lupiáñez J, Tudela P. The three attentional networks: on their independence and interactions. Brain Cogn 2004;54:225-227.
crossref pmid
18. Fan J, Gu X, Guise KG, Liu X, Fossella J, Wang H, et al. Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of attentional networks. Brain Cogn 2009;70:209-220.
crossref pmid pmc
19. Fossella J, Posner MI, Fan J, Swanson JM, Pfaff DW. Attentional phenotypes for the analysis of higher mental function. Sci World J 2002;2:217-223.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
20. Wang K, Fan J, Dong Y, Wang CQ, Lee TM, Posner MI. Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005;78(2-3):235-241.
crossref pmid
21. Han G, Klimes-Dougan B, Jepsen S, Ballard K, Nelson M, Houri A, et al. Selective neurocognitive impairments in adolescents with major depressive disorder. J Adolesc 2012;35:11-20.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
22. Johnson KA, Robertson IH, Barry E, Mulligan A, Dáibhis A, Daly M, et al. Impaired conflict resolution and alerting in children with ADHD: evidence from the Attention Network Task (ANT). J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008;49:1339-1347.
crossref pmid
23. Fan J, Bernardi S, Van Dam NT, Anagnostou E, Gu X, Martin L, et al. Functional deficits of the attentional networks in autism. Brain Behav 2012;2:647-660.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
24. Keehn B, Lincoln AJ, Muller RA, Townsend J. Attentional networks in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2010;51:1251-1259.
crossref pmid pmc
25. Ramaswami G, Geschwind DH. Genetics of autism spectrum disorder. Handb Clin Neurol 2018;147:321-329.
crossref pmid
26. Ronald A, Hoekstra RA. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2011;156b:255-274.
crossref pmid
27. Chien YL, Wu YY, Chiu YN, Liu SK, Tsai WC, Lin PI, et al. Association study of the CNS patterning genes and autism in Han Chinese in Taiwan. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011;35:1512-1517.
crossref pmid
28. Wilson LB, Tregellas JR, Slason E, Pasko BE, Hepburn S, Rojas DC. Phonological processing in first-degree relatives of individuals with autism: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 2013;34:1447-1463.
crossref pmid pmc pdf
29. Shivers CM. Empathy and perceptions of their brother or sister among adolescent siblings of individuals with and without autism spectrum disorder. Res Dev Disabil 2019;92:103451
crossref pmid
30. Losh M, Adolphs R, Poe MD, Couture S, Penn D, Baranek GT, et al. Neuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotype. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66:518-526.
crossref pmid pmc
31. Sucksmith E, Roth I, Hoekstra RA. Autistic traits below the clinical threshold: re-examining the broader autism phenotype in the 21st century. Neuropsychol Rev 2011;21:360-389.
crossref pmid pdf
32. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed). 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

33. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;53:695-699.
crossref pmid
34. Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale. 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.

35. Spreen O, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms and commentary. 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998.

36. Zhang L, Sun Y, Chen F, Wu D, Tang J, Han X, et al. Psychometric properties of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient in both clinical and non-clinical samples: Chinese version for mainland China. BMC Psychiatry 2016;16:213
crossref pmid pmc
37. Keehn B, Joseph RM. Impaired prioritization of novel onset stimuli in autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008;49:1296-1303.
crossref pmid
38. Ciesielski KT, Courchesne E, Elmasian R. Effects of focused selective attention tasks on event-related potentials in autistic and normal individuals. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990;75:207-220.
crossref pmid
39. Murakami Y, Sakai S, Takeda K, Sawamura D, Yoshida K, Hirose T, et al. Autistic traits modulate the activity of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in response to female faces. Neurosci Res 2018;133:28-37.
crossref pmid
40. McDowell S, Whyte J, D’Esposito M. Differential effect of a dopaminergic agonist on prefrontal function in traumatic brain injury patients. Brain 1998;121:1155-1164.
crossref pmid
41. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci 2012;35:73-89.
crossref pmid pmc
42. Greenaway R, Plaisted K. Top-down attentional modulation in autistic spectrum disorders is stimulus-specific. Psychol Sci 2005;16:987-994.
crossref pmid pdf
43. Renner P, Grofer Klinger L, Klinger MR. Exogenous and endogenous attention orienting in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol 2006;12:361-382.
crossref pmid
44. Jones EJH, Dawson G, Webb SJ. Sensory hypersensitivity predicts enhanced attention capture by faces in the early development of ASD. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2018;29:11-20.
crossref pmid pmc
45. Hughes C, Leboyer M, Bouvard M. Executive function in parents of children with autism. Psychol Med 1997;27:209-220.
crossref pmid
46. Hill EL. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Dev Rev 2004;24:189-233.
crossref
47. Hughes C, Plumet MH, Leboyer M. Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40:705-718.
crossref pmid pdf
48. Wong D, Maybery M, Bishop DV, Maley A, Hallmayer J. Profiles of executive function in parents and siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Genes Brain Behav 2006;5:561-576.
crossref pmid


ABOUT
AUTHOR INFORMATION
ARTICLE CATEGORY

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
Editorial Office
#522, 27, Seochojungang-ro 24-gil, Seocho-gu, Seoul 06601, Korea
Tel: +82-2-717-0892    E-mail: psychiatryinvest@gmail.com                

Copyright © 2024 by Korean Neuropsychiatric Association.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next