Childhood Trauma and Depressive Level Among Chinese College Students in Guangzhou: The Roles of Rumination and Perceived Stress

Article information

Psychiatry Investig. 2024;21(4):352-360
Publication date (electronic) : 2024 April 23
doi : https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2023.0188
Department of Applied Psychology, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, Guangzhou, China
Correspondence: Junjing Wang, PhD Department of Applied Psychology, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, Guangzhou 510006, China Tel: +86-15521430628, E-mail: lwwangjunjing@126.com
Correspondence: Qian Liang, MS Department of Applied Psychology, Guangdong University of Foreign Studies, Guangzhou 510006, China Tel: +86-15073490957, E-mail: liangqian0106@qq.com
Received 2023 June 28; Revised 2023 October 19; Accepted 2024 January 1.

Abstract

Objective

Although previous studies have validated the effect of childhood trauma on depressive level, few studies have utilized the diathesis-stress theory to investigate the specific roles of perceived stress and rumination in the pathway between childhood trauma and depression in Chinese college students. This study aims to demonstrate the mediation effect of perceived stress and the moderation effect of rumination in the pathway between childhood trauma and depressive level in Chinese college students.

Methods

A total of 995 Chinese college students in Guangzhou were included in this study by recruitment advertisement from October to December 2021. And they were asked to finish four self-report questionnaires, including Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form, Perceived Stress Scale, the 22-item Ruminative Response Scale, and Beck Depression Scale-II. Then the data were analyzed with Mplus 8.3.

Results

Results revealed significant correlations among childhood trauma, perceived stress, rumination and depressive level. Further analyses revealed that perceived stress played a mediation role between childhood trauma and depressive level (estimate=0.09, standard error [SE]=0.02, t=5.93, 95% confidence interval [CI]=0.06–0.12), and rumination played a moderation role between childhood trauma and perceived stress (estimate=-0.17, SE=0.06, t=-2.86, 95% CI=-0.28– -0.05]) as well as between childhood trauma and depressive level (estimate=0.10, SE=0.04, t=2.74, 95% CI=0.03–0.16).

Conclusion

These results revealed the mediation effect of perceived stress and the moderation effect of rumination in the pathway between childhood trauma and depressive level in Chinese college students, which helped us to understand how the childhood trauma influenced the depressive level and gave us multi-dimensional indications for reducing the effect of childhood trauma on depressive level.

INTRODUCTION

Depression is one of the most common causes of disability around the world. As reported by World Health Organization, it is estimated that more than 350 million people suffer from it [1]. For college students, depression is also one of the most common mental disorders [2], which is also prevalent in China [3,4]. Depression in college students may influence their academic attainments and interpersonal relationship [5], and worse, may lead to suicide [6]. Therefore, given the high prevalence and serious consequences of depression, it is necessary to identify its influential factors among college students.

In college students, previous studies [3,7] have validated that childhood trauma is correlated with the risk of depression, and that childhood trauma could predict depression [8-10]. For the mechanism of this effect, some studies [11,12] found that individuals who experienced trauma were more sensitive to future adversity or negative events. Thus, minor adverse events may also trigger their depressive reactions. In other words, childhood trauma, a distal stressor, would increase individuals’ sensitivity to stress, which would then lower their threshold to suffer from depression [13]. These findings above were consistent with the diathesis-stress theory [14], which suggested that the stress may activate a vulnerability to trigger depressive symptoms. Thus, the stress may play a vital role in the relationship between childhood trauma and depressive level.

College students are usually under varying kinds of pressure, such as continuing study pressure, family pressure, or employment pressure [15,16]. But high levels of stress also could affect students’ interest in learning, general health, physical function [17] and even increase their depressive levels [18-20]. Consistent with the theory of diathesis-stress mentioned above, many studies [21-23] have suggested that individuals who have adversity experiences in childhood may have higher perceived stress levels, which might increase the risk of depression. Further, some studies [22,24-27] have validated the mediation role of stress between different types of childhood trauma and depressive level in various populations. However, few studies focused on the mediation role of perceived stress between childhood trauma and depressive level among Chinese college students. Perceived stress refers to a subjective evaluation and assessment of the stress level for an objective event that individuals experienced [28]. Previous studies [20,29] suggested that individuals with higher perceived stress levels would report more depressive symptoms than those with lower perceived stress levels when faced with the same stress event, which means that perceived stress may determine the severity and impact of stress.

Nevertheless, some studies [9,30] discovered that individuals who had childhood traumatic experiences may not report more symptoms of depression. Thus, there may be a moderation factor in the relationship between childhood trauma and depression. Rumination was defined as ‘the process of thinking about one’s negative and distressing feelings and bad consequences repeatedly and passively, rather than actively solving problems’ [31]. Previous studies have validated that rumination was correlated with childhood trauma and depression, especially the positive correlation between high rumination level and childhood traumatic experiences [32-34], as well as between high rumination level and high depressive level [35]. Further, previous studies [36,37] have found that rumination mediated the relationship between childhood trauma and depression, that is higher childhood traumatic experiences lead to higher depressive levels via increasing their rumination levels. In total, these studies above emphasized the positive correlations between high rumination level with childhood traumatic experience and depression, while the correlations between low rumination level with childhood traumatic experience and depression were not clear. In other words, it remains unknown whether high and low rumination levels can bring different effects of childhood trauma on depression. Thus, it is necessary to investigate the moderation role of rumination in the pathway between childhood trauma and depressive level to compare the roles of high and low rumination level.

Based on the diathesis-stress theory and the above studies, this study aimed to set up a comprehensive model to investigate the possible pathway between childhood trauma and depressive level in non-clinical Chinese college students, focusing on the roles of perceived stress and rumination. Specifically, this study hypothesized that perceived stress played a mediation role in the pathway between childhood trauma and depressive level (hypothesis 1), and rumination played a moderation role in the pathway between childhood trauma and depressive level (hypothesis 2). We illustrated our theoretical model using Figure 1 as a guide.

Figure 1.

The theoretical model used in this study.

METHODS

Participants

The sample size in this questionnaire survey study was determined based on Monte Carlo simulation and the possible rate of invalid answers. Specifically, Monte Carlo simulation suggested that 702 participants were needed to obtain at least 0.80 of the statistical power. Considering the possible rate of 20% for the invalid records, a sample size of 883 participants was recommended. With the help of our collaborators, 1,212 college students without any clinical diagnosis of psychiatric diseases (non-clinical) were recruited from October to December 2021 in Guangzhou, China, via advertisement.

This study was approved by the ethical committee of the School of Public Administration in Guangdong University of Foreign Studies (file number: 20230605). Written informed consent was obtained from each participant, and all participants were asked to fulfill four questionnaires according to their real situation and were paid for their participation. One participant would be excluded from this study if they met one of these following criteria: 1) the answer time was below the average time (4 min), 2) the answers to the items conflicted with each other obviously, 3) the answers to the polygraph questions were completely inconsistent (the polygraph question was set up to repeat an item at a certain interval of time to test whether the participants’ choice was consistent). Eventually, 995 participants (306 males and 689 females) were included in this study and participants were between 19 and 26 years old (mean=19.89; standard deviation [SD]=1.99). The flow diagram of the participant inclusion process in this study was displayed in Supplementary Figure 1 (in the online-only Data Supplement).

Measures

Childhood trauma

The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form (CTQ-SF) was used to evaluate traumatic experiences before 16 years old [38]. The scale was a 28-item (including 25 clinical items and 3 validity items) self-report scale which contained five subscales: emotional neglect, physical neglect, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, and physical abuse. Each item was presented in a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). The total score of this scale ranged from 25 to 125, with higher score indicating greater level of abuse or neglect experienced by the participant. The Chinese version of CTQ-SF has been proven to have high internal reliability and test–retest reliability [9,39]. The Cronbach’s alpha of the CTQ-SF in this study was 0.87.

Perceived stress

The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) was used to assess the college students’ perceived stress in the last month [28]. It was a self-report scale including 14 items and adopted a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (almost never) to 4 (almost always). The total score of this score ranged from 0 to 56, with higher score indicating greater level of perceived stress of the participant [40]. The Chinese version of PSS has been validated to be a reliable and valid measurement in rating stress among college students [41]. Its Cronbach’s alpha coefficient in this study was 0.86.

Rumination

The 22-item Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS-22), a self-report rumination measure, was mainly used to measure how often one participant think or do each item when they are feeling down, sad, or depressed (e.g., “Think why do I have problems other people do not have?”) [42]. The scale consisted of 22 items, which was rated on a 4-point Likert scaling from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always), with higher score indicating higher frequency of ruminative tendencies [43]. Previous studies [44,45] have shown that the Chinese version of the RRS-22 has excellent convergent validity and internal consistency. Its Cronbach’s alpha in this study was 0.93.

Depressive level

The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II), a widely used measurement of depressive symptoms, was primarily used to rate participants’ depressive symptoms and their severity in the past seven days [46,47]. The scale consisted of 21 items with a 4-point response scale ranging from 0 (almost never) to 3 (almost always). The total score of this scale ranged from 0 to 63 with higher score indicating higher severity of depression [46]. Previous studies [48,49] have demonstrated that the Chinese version of the BDI-II has excellent reliability and validity. The Cronbach’s alpha of BDI-II in this study was 0.88.

Statistical analysis

Data analyses were conducted with Mplus 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA) with the following steps. First of all, descriptive analyses and correlation analyses were used to reveal the relationship among all variables. Then, moderated mediation analyses were conducted to test the mediation effect of perceived stress on the relationship between childhood trauma and depressive level, and the moderation effect of rumination on the relationship between childhood trauma and perceived stress as well as between childhood trauma and depressive level. Considering that sex and age were common influential factors for depression [50,51] and were always regarded as covariates in analyses [52,53], Sex (0=female, 1=male) and age were also controlled for their effects on depressive level in this study. In addition, childhood trauma and rumination were grand-mean centered before creating the interactive term [54]. The maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard error (SE) was utilized to take into account the possible influence of non-normal data distribution on the parameter estimation [55]. Besides, 95% bias-corrected confidence interval (CI) with 10,000 resampling were obtained for all of the parameter estimates. Pseudo R2 were reported for perceived stress and depressive level.

Supplementary analyses

In order to test whether the theoretical model in this study can be applied in different subtypes of childhood trauma, we also conducted the above analyses based on each subtype of childhood trauma, respectively.

RESULTS

Common method bias

All variables in this study were collected by self-report questionnaires which may pose the risk of common method bias. Therefore, Harman’s single factor test was used to conduct exploratory factor analyses on all variables. The results extracted 22 factors with the eigenvalue greater than 1. The maximum factor variance explained 21.52%, which was far less than 40%. Thus, the influence of common method bias on the results of this study was not significant.

Descriptive analyses

As shown in Table 1, significant correlations were found among childhood trauma, perceived stress, rumination, and depressive level. Specifically, childhood trauma was positively correlated with depressive level (r=0.05, p<0.01), perceived stress (r=0.05, p<0.01), and rumination (r=0.04, p<0.01). Meanwhile, depressive level was positively correlated with perceived stress (r=0.15, p<0.01), and rumination (r=0.11, p<0.01). Perceived stress was also positively correlated with rumination (r=0.16, p<0.01). Other correlations could be found in Table 1.

Means, SD, and correlations of all variables in this study (N=995)

Hypothesis testing

As shown in Table 2, childhood trauma significantly impacted perceived stress (estimate=0.15, SE=0.04, t=4.03, 95% CI=0.06–0.22), whereas perceived stress significantly affected depressive level (estimate=0.32, SE=0.02, t=18.95, 95% CI=0.29–0.36). Perceived stress positively mediated the relationship between childhood trauma and depressive level (estimate=0.09, SE=0.02, t=5.93, 95% CI=0.06–0.12), and the direct influence of perceived stress on depressive level remained significant (estimate=0.13, SE=0.02, t=5.54, 95% CI=0.09–0.18).

The moderated mediation effect of childhood trauma on depressive level

Rumination negatively moderated the relationship between childhood trauma and perceived stress (estimate=-0.17, SE=0.06, t=-2.86, 95% CI=-0.28– -0.05]). Further simple analysis suggested that, the relationship between childhood trauma and perceived stress (estimate=0.05, SE=0.05, t=1.15, 95% CI=-0.04–0.14) and the mediation effect of perceived stress between childhood trauma and depressive level (estimate=0.02, SE=0.02, t=1.16, 95% CI=-0.01–0.05) were not significant when rumination was one standard deviation above its mean. However, these two relationships were significant when rumination was one standard deviation below its mean. Specifically, the relationship between childhood trauma and perceived stress (estimate=0.24, SE=0.05, t=4.66, 95% CI=0.14–0.34) and the mediation effect of perceived stress between childhood trauma and depressive level (estimate=0.08, SE=0.02, t=4.50, 95% CI=0.04–0.11) were significant and stronger when rumination was one standard deviation below its mean. The results of simple analyses were shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Rumination moderated the effect of childhood trauma (A) on perceived stress and (B) on depressive level. SD, standard deviations.

With the direct influence of trauma on depressive level, nonetheless, the moderating effect of rumination was positive (estimate=0.10, SE=0.04, t=2.74, 95% CI=0.03–0.16), in which the relationship was stronger (estimate=0.16, SE=0.03, t=5.26, 95% CI=0.10–0.22) when rumination was one standard deviation above its mean, and weaker (estimate=0.06, SE=0.03, t=2.18, 95% CI=0.01–0.10) when rumination was one standard deviation below its mean.

Supplementary results

The results based on each subtype of childhood trauma were displayed in Supplementary Tables 1-4 and Supplementary Figure 1 (in the online-only Data Supplement). Results revealed that the theoretical model in this study was especially valid in the subtype of emotional neglect.

DISCUSSION

This study constructed a moderated mediation model to analyze the possible pathways between childhood trauma and depressive level in non-clinical Chinese college students with considering the mediation role of perceived stress and the moderation role of rumination. Results first revealed that childhood trauma has a negative effect on depression level, and perceived stress mediated this effect. These results supported the hypothesis 1. Results also indicated that rumination moderated the influence of childhood trauma on perceived stress and on depressive level, which supported the hypothesis 2.

As predicted, firstly, this study validated the negative effect of childhood trauma on depressive level in college students (Tables 1 and 2), which means that those with childhood traumatic experiences were more likely to report high depressive levels. This result in Chinese college students was consistent with prior studies focusing on other populations, such as college students [10,27,56], adolescents [57], adults [58-60], elders [61], and clinical patients [62]. The possible reason for this maybe that individuals with traumatic experiences possessed more negative emotions, distorted cognition or personality deficits, which might lead to a social and psychological vulnerability in the future [63]. Thus, the childhood trauma may be a stable and significant predictive factor of the prevalence of depression, which required us to pay more attention to the effects of childhood trauma on depression and further take measures to prevent and intervene depression.

Meanwhile, our results revealed that perceived stress mediated the relationship between childhood trauma and depressive level (Table 2), which was also in line with previous studies [27,64]. These results indicated that childhood trauma would result in individuals’ depression by enhancing their sensitivity to stress or heightening their perceived stress levels [65,66]. The potential reason for this maybe that high sensitivity to stress was functional adaptation to childhood trauma, constituting a potential mechanism by which childhood trauma would induce some mental illness, including anxiety and depression disorders [65,67]. These findings could be explained by the theory of diathesis-stress, proposing that psychological or social stress could act on a vulnerability to trigger depressive symptoms [26]. In other words, childhood trauma hindered the development of children’s self-structuring and enhanced their psychological susceptibility to future stressors, which would lead to more depressive symptoms [68]. In total, our results revealed that childhood trauma affected depression not only directly but also indirectly through perceived stress, which reminded us to pay more attention to the perceived stress when taking measures to alleviate the effects of childhood trauma on depression.

In addition, this study also discovered that rumination not only moderated the link between childhood trauma and perceived stress (Figure 2A and Table 2) but also moderated the association between childhood trauma and depressive level (Figure 2B and Table 2). Rumination was characterized by repetitive negative thinking, and previous studies have revealed its significant correlations with childhood trauma [32-34] and depressive level [35,69]. In our study, on one hand, we detected the moderation role of rumination between childhood trauma and perceived stress (Figure 2A). Specifically, childhood trauma had significant and strong effect on perceived stress when the rumination level was low, while childhood trauma has no significant effect on perceived stress when the rumination level was high. And, importantly, the perceived stress based on high rumination level were much larger than low rumination level no matter the childhood trauma is high or low. These results indicated that compared with childhood trauma, rumination may have a greater negative impact on perceived stress, which has also been evidenced in previous studies. A study [70] found that the adolescents with low levels of rumination would report perceived stress below the average levels. Also, another study [71] found that rumination was still a great risk factor of perceived stress, even after controlling for multiple potential factors, such as sex, age and some physical diseases. Of note, an intervention study [72] on rumination have validated that individuals could alleviate their stress reactivity by reducing their levels of rumination. Overall, this study has showed that rumination has a great impact on the perceived stress levels of college students and can increase the negative indirect effect of childhood trauma on depressive level. On the other hand, we discovered the moderation role of rumination between childhood trauma and depressive level (Figure 2B). Specifically, childhood trauma had significant and strong effect on depressive level when the rumination was high, while the childhood trauma had significant but weak effect on depressive level when the rumination was low. These indicated that higher childhood traumatic experiences would result in more depressive symptoms, and the high rumination level would strengthen this effect. The results were in line with previous studies [73,74] which showed that childhood trauma could significantly affect depressive symptoms and further increase the likelihood of suffering from depression. Further, the possible reason for the strengthening effect of rumination may be that for individuals with higher childhood traumatic experiences, relatively higher rumination levels will make them more worried about their status quo (e.g., their traumatic experiences and the corresponding consequence), thus increasing the risk and severity of depression [75]. Overall, this study has validated that rumination had a great negative effect on college students’ depressive levels, and can moderate the direct effect of childhood trauma on depressive level. To sum up, these findings indicated the significance of rumination for mental health among college students and further gave us indications that more actions should be taken to alleviate their levels of rumination.

In general, in theory, our study revealed a pathway on how childhood trauma affects the depressive level of college students, by combining a mediation effect of perceived stress and the moderation effect of rumination, based on a moderated mediation model. In practice, our study gave implications to alleviate depressive levels of non-clinical Chinese college students, especially for those with childhood traumatic experience. First, parents or caregivers should try their best to reduce the children’s traumatic experience, such as giving them love and respect, rather than discrimination, abuse or corporal punishment. Second, for those with childhood traumatic experiences, we could prevent and reduce students’ depressive levels by alleviating their perceived stress levels. Specifically, we could take some actions to improve students’ ability to cope with stress (e.g., encouraging them to participate community activity and competitions), which would not only help reduce their perceived stress levels, but also further alleviate their depressive symptoms. Third, we should pay more attention to college students’ level of rumination and take some other interventions to reduce their rumination levels, such as mindfulness, attention control exercises, cognitive reappraisal, and cognitive control training [34,76,77].

Limitations

There were still some limitations in this study. Firstly, because this study is based on a cross-sectional design, it cannot reveal the causal relationship among childhood trauma, rumination, perceived stress, and depressive level. Therefore, a future longitudinal study should be conducted to verify the causal relationship among the above variables. Secondly, this study only recruited non-clinical participants, and it was unclear whether our results can be applied in clinical patients. Future researches should recruit clinical depressive patients to test the replicability of this model in our study. Thirdly, this study only included college students in Guangzhou as participants, which may limit the generalization of our conclusions. In future studies, we should extend the sampling in order to validate our findings. Fourthly, supplementary results indicated that the theoretical model was especially valid in the subtype of emotional neglect, which emphasized the role of perceived stress and rumination in the pathway between emotional neglect and depressive level. Future studies should investigate which factors play more important roles in the pathway between each of other subtypes and depressive level. Lastly, the data in this study were collected in 2021 and the manuscript was submitted in 2023, which indicate the lag of time for this study. Future studies should use the timely data and validate the conclusions in this study.

Conclusion

In general, this study revealed a potential pathway of how childhood trauma affects the depressive level among nonclinical college students. The findings further give us multi-dimensional (perceived stress, and rumination among college students) indications for reducing the negative effects of childhood trauma on depressive level, which will be helpful for improving their mental health.

Supplementary materials

The online-only Data Supplement is available with this article at https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2023.0188.

Supplementary Table 1.

Means, SD, and correlations of variables in this study when using the subtypes of childhood trauma (i.e., emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect and physical neglect) (N=995)

pi-2023-0188-Supplementary-Table-1.pdf
Supplementary Table 2.

Direct and indirect effect of a subtype of childhood trauma on depressive level, controlling for other subtypes of childhood trauma, sex and age

pi-2023-0188-Supplementary-Table-2.pdf
Supplementary Table 3.

The moderating effect of rumination on the relationships between a subtype of childhood trauma and perceived stress, and depressive level, controlling for other subtypes of childhood trauma, sex, and age

pi-2023-0188-Supplementary-Table-3.pdf
Supplementary Table 4.

The simple effect of a subtype of childhood trauma on depressive level, controlling for other subtypes of childhood trauma, sex, and age

pi-2023-0188-Supplementary-Table-4.pdf
Supplementary Figure 1.

The simple effect of a subtype of childhood trauma on depressive level, controlling for other subtypes of childhood trauma, sex, and age

pi-2023-0188-Supplementary-Figure-1.pdf

Notes

Availability of Data and Material

The datasets generated or analyzed during the study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Junjing Wang. Data curation: Qian Liang, Yueqi Ma, Yi Zhang. Formal analysis: An Yang, Qian Liang, Yueqi Ma, Yi Zhang. Funding acquisition: Junjing Wang, Yi Zhang. Investigation: Junjing Wang. Methodology: An Yang, Qian Liang, Yueqi Ma, Yi Zhang. Project administration: Junjing Wang. Resources: Junjing Wang. Software: An Yang, Qian Liang. Supervision: Junjing Wang. Validation: Qian Liang, Yueqi Ma, Yi Zhang. Visualization: Qian Liang. Writing—original draft: Qian Liang. Writing—review & editing: Junjing Wang, An Yang.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the Guangdong Province university characteristic innovation project (2023KTSCX032), and the Guangdong University of Foreign Studies Graduate Research Innovation Project (21GWCXXM-050 and 22GWCXXM-056). Philosophy and Social Science Planning Project of Guangdong Province (GD24YJY07).

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the fundings that support our study and the participants involved in this study.

References

1. Smith K. Mental health: a world of depression. Nature 2014;515:181.
2. Auerbach RP, Mortier P, Bruffaerts R, Alonso J, Benjet C, Cuijpers P, et al. WHO World Mental Health Surveys International College Student Project: Prevalence and distribution of mental disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 2018;127:623–638.
3. Gao L, Xie Y, Jia C, Wang W. Prevalence of depression among Chinese university students: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep 2020;10:15897.
4. Ibrahim AK, Kelly SJ, Adams CE, Glazebrook C. A systematic review of studies of depression prevalence in university students. J Psychiatr Res 2013;47:391–400.
5. Cox DW, Kealy D, Kahn JH, McCloskey KD, Joyce AS, Ogrodniczuk JS. Depression symptoms’ impact on personality disorder treatment: depression symptoms amplifying the interpersonal benefits of negative-affect expression. J Affect Disord 2020;272:318–325.
6. Ho TC, Walker JC, Teresi GI, Kulla A, Kirshenbaum JS, Gifuni AJ, et al. Default mode and salience network alterations in suicidal and nonsuicidal self-injurious thoughts and behaviors in adolescents with depression. Transl Psychiatry 2021;11:38.
7. Tao J, He K, Xu J. The mediating effect of self-compassion on the relationship between childhood maltreatment and depression. J Affect Disord 2021;291:288–293.
8. Chu Q, Wang X, Yao R, Fan J, Li Y, Nie F, et al. Childhood trauma and current depression among Chinese university students: a moderated mediation model of cognitive emotion regulation strategies and neuroticism. Bmc Psychiatry 2022;22:90.
9. Chang JJ, Ji Y, Li YH, Yuan MY, Su PY. Childhood trauma and depression in college students: mediating and moderating effects of psychological resilience. Asian J Psychiatr 2021;65:102824.
10. Mall S, Mortier P, Taljaard L, Roos J, Stein DJ, Lochner C. The relationship between childhood adversity, recent stressors, and depression in college students attending a South African university. Bmc Psychiatry 2018;18:63.
11. Kendler KS, Kuhn JW, Prescott CA. Childhood sexual abuse, stressful life events and risk for major depression in women. Psychol Med 2004;34:1475–1482.
12. LeMoult J, Humphreys KL, Tracy A, Hoffmeister JA, Ip E, Gotlib IH. Meta-analysis: exposure to early life stress and risk for depression in childhood and adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2020;59:842–855.
13. Heim C, Newport DJ, Mletzko T, Miller AH, Nemeroff CB. The link between childhood trauma and depression: insights from HPA axis studies in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2008;33:693–710.
14. Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull 1991;110:406–425.
15. Acharya L, Jin L, Collins W. College life is stressful today - emerging stressors and depressive symptoms in college students. J Am Coll Health 2018;66:655–664.
16. Maykrantz SA, Houghton JD. Self-leadership and stress among college students: examining the moderating role of coping skills. J Am Coll Health 2020;68:89–96.
17. Heruti I, Levy S, Avitsur R. Physical injury, health, and well-being: role of stress perception. Injury 2018;49:1546–1551.
18. Brown GW, Bifulco A, Harris TO. Life events, vulnerability and onset of depression: some refinements. Br J Psychiatry 1987;150:30–42.
19. Slavich GM, Irwin MR. From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: a social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychol Bull 2014;140:774–815.
20. Hammen C. Stress and depression. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2005;1:293–319.
21. Mishra AA, Marceau K. Co-occurring childhood maltreatment exposure and depressive symptoms in adulthood: testing differential effects of stress dysregulation and perceived stress. Aging Ment Health 2020;24:1837–1846.
22. Sousa C, Mason WA, Herrenkohl TI, Prince D, Herrenkohl RC, Russo MJ. Direct and indirect effects of child abuse and environmental stress: a lifecourse perspective on adversity and depressive symptoms. Am J Orthopsychiatry 2018;88:180–188.
23. Weissman DG, Lambert HK, Rodman AM, Peverill M, Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Reduced hippocampal and amygdala volume as a mechanism underlying stress sensitization to depression following childhood trauma. Depress Anxiety 2020;37:916–925.
24. Mayer SE, Surachman A, Prather AA, Puterman E, Delucchi KL, Irwin MR, et al. The long shadow of childhood trauma for depression in midlife: examining daily psychological stress processes as a persistent risk pathway. Psychol Med 2021;52:4029–4038.
25. Vranceanu AM, Hobfoll SE, Johnson RJ. Child multi-type maltreatment and associated depression and PTSD symptoms: the role of social support and stress. Child Abuse Negl 2007;31:71–84.
26. Zheng K, Chu J, Zhang X, Ding Z, Song Q, Liu Z, et al. Psychological resilience and daily stress mediate the effect of childhood trauma on depression. Child Abuse Negl 2022;125:105485.
27. Kelifa MO, Yang Y, Herbert C, He Q, Wang P. Psychological resilience and current stressful events as potential mediators between adverse childhood experiences and depression among college students in Eritrea. Child Abuse Negl 2020;106:104480.
28. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 1983;24:385–396.
29. Ravindran AV, Matheson K, Griffiths J, Merali Z, Anisman H. Stress, coping, uplifts, and quality of life in subtypes of depression: a conceptual frame and emerging data. J Affect Disord 2002;71:121–130.
30. Watters ER, Aloe AM, Wojciak AS. Examining the associations between childhood trauma, resilience, and depression: a multivariate meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse 2023;24:231–244.
31. Nolen-Hoeksema S. Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. J Abnorm Psychol 1991;100:569–582.
32. Deguchi A, Masuya J, Naruse M, Morishita C, Higashiyama M, Tanabe H, et al. Rumination mediates the effects of childhood maltreatment and trait anxiety on depression in non-clinical adult volunteers. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2021;17:3439–3445.
33. Mansueto G, Cavallo C, Palmieri S, Ruggiero GM, Sassaroli S, Caselli G. Adverse childhood experiences and repetitive negative thinking in adulthood: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychother 2021;28:557–568.
34. Muñoz RT, Hanks H. A structural model of adverse childhood experiences as antecedents of rumination leading to lower hope. J Interpers Violence 2021;36:9790–9807.
35. Ruscio AM, Gentes EL, Jones JD, Hallion LS, Coleman ES, Swendsen J. Rumination predicts heightened responding to stressful life events in major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2015;124:17–26.
36. Kim JS, Jin MJ, Jung W, Hahn SW, Lee SH. Rumination as a mediator between childhood trauma and adulthood depression/anxiety in nonclinical participants. Front Psychol 2017;8:1597.
37. Raes F, Hermans D. On the mediating role of subtypes of rumination in the relationship between childhood emotional abuse and depressed mood: brooding versus reflection. Depress Anxiety 2008;25:1067–1070.
38. Bernstein DP, Stein JA, Newcomb MD, Walker E, Pogge D, Ahluvalia T, et al. Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse Negl 2003;27:169–190.
39. He J, Zhong X, Gao Y, Xiong G, Yao S. Psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire-Short Form (CTQ-SF) among undergraduates and depressive patients. Child Abuse Negl 2019;91:102–108.
40. Remor E. Psychometric properties of a European Spanish version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS). Span J Psychol 2006;9:86–93.
41. Huang F, Wang H, Wang Z, Zhang J, Du W, Su C, et al. Psychometric properties of the perceived stress scale in a community sample of Chinese. BMC Psychiatry 2020;20:130.
42. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. J Pers Soc Psychol 1991;61:115–121.
43. Schoofs H, Hermans D, Raes F. Brooding and reflection as subtypes of rumination: Evidence from confirmatory factor analysis in nonclinical samples using the Dutch Ruminative Response Scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2010;32:609–617.
44. Liu M, Jiang Y, Wang X, Liu Q, Wu H. The role of rumination and stressful life events in the relationship between the qi stagnation constitution and depression in women: a moderated mediation model. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med 2017;2017:7605893.
45. Wang Q, Zhu XZ, Yi JY. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of rumination response scale in depressed patients. Nerv Dis Ment Health 2011;11:544–546.
46. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri WF. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess 1996;67:588–597.
47. Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. The validity of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. An updated literature review. J Psychosom Res 2002;52:69–77.
48. Sun XY, Li YX, Yu CQ, Li LM. [Reliability and validity of depression scales of Chinese version: a systematic review]. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi 2017;38:110–116. Chinese.
49. Wang J, Lai R, Yang A, Yang M, Guo Y. Helicopter parenting and depressive level among non-clinical Chinese college students: a moderated mediation model. J Affect Disord 2021;295:522–529.
50. Luppa M, Sikorski C, Luck T, Ehreke L, Konnopka A, Wiese B, et al. Age- and gender-specific prevalence of depression in latest-life–systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 2012;136:212–221.
51. Vallis EH, Stephens M, Ross B, Rempel S, Howard C, Liu D, et al. Depressive symptoms in youth before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: longitudinal investigation of patterns dependent on age, sex, and family history of mental illness. Psychol Med 2024;54:895–901.
52. Šagud M, Janović MB, Ćusa ZV, Jakšić N, Krakan LB, Begić D, et al. Depression and stress levels in patients with different psychiatric disorders during concurrent early-phase COVID-19 pandemic and earthquake in Croatia. BMC Psychiatry 2023;23:798.
53. Kassel MT, Kryza-Lacombe M, Insel PS, Rhodes E, Satre DD, Nelson JC, et al. Hoarding symptoms in late life depression are associated with greater executive dysfunction and disability and poorer response to depression treatment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2024;32:137–147.
54. Enders CK, Tofighi D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: a new look at an old issue. Psychol Methods 2007;12:121–138.
55. Yuan KH, Bentler PM. 5. Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis with nonnormal missing data. Sociol Methodol 2000;30:165–200.
56. Zhang Y, Liu J, Chen L, Yang T, Luo X, Cui X, et al. Prevalence of cooccurring severe depression and psychotic symptoms in college students and its relationship with childhood maltreatment. Child Abuse Negl 2023;146:106470.
57. Copeland WE, Alaie I, Jonsson U, Shanahan L. Associations of childhood and adolescent depression with adult psychiatric and functional outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2021;60:604–611.
58. Poole JC, Dobson KS, Pusch D. Childhood adversity and adult depression: the protective role of psychological resilience. Child Abuse Negl 2017;64:89–100.
59. Sheikh MA. Child maltreatment, psychopathological symptoms, and onset of diabetes mellitus, hypothyroidism and COPD in adulthood. J Affect Disord 2018;241:80–85.
60. Stern KR, Thayer ZM. Adversity in childhood and young adulthood predicts young adult depression. Int J Public Health 2019;64:1069–1074.
61. Xiang X, Wang X. Childhood adversity and major depression in later life: a competing-risks regression analysis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2021;36:215–223.
62. Huh HJ, Kim KH, Lee HK, Chae JH. The relationship between childhood trauma and the severity of adulthood depression and anxiety symptoms in a clinical sample: the mediating role of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. J Affect Disord 2017;213:44–50.
63. Wang D, Lu S, Gao W, Wei Z, Duan J, Hu S, et al. The impacts of childhood trauma on psychosocial features in a Chinese sample of young adults. Psychiatry Investig 2018;15:1046–1052.
64. Hazel NA, Hammen C, Brennan PA, Najman J. Early childhood adversity and adolescent depression: the mediating role of continued stress. Psychol Med 2008;38:581–589.
65. Betz LT, Penzel N, Rosen M, Kambeitz J. Relationships between childhood trauma and perceived stress in the general population: a network perspective. Psychol Med 2021;51:2696–2706.
66. Seitz R, Vracotas N, Bechard-Evans L, King S, Abadi S, Joober R, et al. The Trier Social Stress Test in first episode psychosis patients: impact of perceived stress, protective factors and childhood trauma. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2019;105:155–163.
67. Isvoranu AM, van Borkulo CD, Boyette LL, Wigman JT, Vinkers CH, Borsboom D, et al. A network approach to psychosis: pathways between childhood trauma and psychotic symptoms. Schizophr Bull 2017;43:187–196.
68. Rocha HA, Sudfeld CR, Leite ÁJ, Rocha SG, Machado MM, Campos JS, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and child development outcomes in Ceara, Brazil: a population-based study. Am J Prev Med 2021;60:79–586.
69. Verstraeten K, Bijttebier P, Vasey MW, Raes F. Specificity of worry and rumination in the development of anxiety and depressive symptoms in children. Br J Clin Psychol 2011;50:364–378.
70. Rushton JL, Forcier M, Schectman RM. Epidemiology of depressive symptoms in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41:199–205.
71. Kovács LN, Baksa D, Dobos D, Eszlári N, Gecse K, Kocsel N, et al. Perceived stress in the time of COVID-19: the association with brooding and COVID-related rumination in adults with and without migraine. BMC Psychol 2021;9:68.
72. Hoorelbeke K, Koster EH, Vanderhasselt MA, Callewaert S, Demeyer I. The influence of cognitive control training on stress reactivity and rumination in response to a lab stressor and naturalistic stress. Behav Res Ther 2015;69:1–10.
73. Husain MI, Umer M, Chaudhry IB, Husain MO, Rahman R, Shakoor S, et al. Relationship between childhood trauma, personality, social support and depression in women attending general medical clinics in a low and middle-income country. J Affect Disord 2021;292:526–533.
74. Racine N, Devereaux C, Cooke JE, Eirich R, Zhu J, Madigan S. Adverse childhood experiences and maternal anxiety and depression: a meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry 2021;21:28.
75. Roley ME, Claycomb MA, Contractor AA, Dranger P, Armour C, Elhai JD. The relationship between rumination, PTSD, and depression symptoms. J Affect Disord 2015;180:116–121.
76. Cohen N, Mor N, Henik A. Linking executive control and emotional response:a training procedure to reduce rumination. Clin Psychol Sci 2015;3:15–25.
77. Pan DN, Hoid D, Gu RL, Li X. Emotional working memory training reduces rumination and alters the EEG microstate in anxious individuals. Neuroimage Clin 2020;28:102488.

Article information Continued

Figure 1.

The theoretical model used in this study.

Figure 2.

Rumination moderated the effect of childhood trauma (A) on perceived stress and (B) on depressive level. SD, standard deviations.

Table 1.

Means, SD, and correlations of all variables in this study (N=995)

Mean±SD 1 2 3 4 5
1. Sex - -
2. Age (yr) 19.89±1.99 -0.29** -
3. Childhood trauma 1.71±0.47 0.02 -0.05 -
4. Depressive level 1.44±0.38 0.01 0.02 0.05** -
5. Perceived stress 2.66±0.59 0.03 0.02 0.05** 0.15** -
6. Rumination 2.24±0.56 0.06** -0.01 0.04** 0.11** 0.16**
**

p<0.01.

SD, standard deviations

Table 2.

The moderated mediation effect of childhood trauma on depressive level

Perceived stress
Depressive level
Estimate SE t 95% CI Estimate SE t 95% CI
Intercept 2.67 0.02 160.74** 2.64–2.70 0.48 0.11 4.57** 0.27–0.69
Sex (0=female, 1=male) -0.01 0.02 -0.65 -0.05–0.03
Age 0.01 0.01 1.42 -0.01–0.02
CT 0.15 0.04 4.03** 0.06–0.22 0.11 0.02 5.35** 0.07–0.15
RU 0.50 0.03 14.79** 0.43–0.56 0.18 0.02 10.01** 0.14–0.21
Perceived stress 0.32 0.02 18.95** 0.29–0.36
CT×RU -0.17 0.06 -2.86** -0.28– -0.05 0.10 0.04 2.74** 0.03–0.16
R2 0.24 0.03 9.01** 0.51 0.02 23.06**

Estimate, unstandardized regression coefficient.

**

p<0.01;

SE, standard error; CI, confidence interval; CT, childhood trauma; RU, rumination; t, value of t-distribution