Psychiatry Investig Search

CLOSE


Psychiatry Investig > Volume 22(3); 2025 > Article
Hong, Lee, Yoo, Choi, and Kim: Reliability and Validity of the Korean Version of the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers: A Comparison Study Between the Clinical and Community Population

Abstract

Objective

This study aimed to evaluate the utility of the Korean version of the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT) in hospital settings and to identify items sensitive for detecting autism spectrum disorder (ASD) trait.

Methods

The Q-CHAT was administered to a clinical sample of children presenting with developmental delays with a high probability of ASD in a hospital setting (n=37), as well as to typically developing community children (n=67), aged 12 to 54 months.

Results

The mean Q-CHAT total score in the hospital sample (42.0±13.6) was significantly higher than in the community sample (29.9±7.8), with maximized sensitivity and adequate specificity at 32.5 (sensitivity=0.811, specificity=0.687). The internal consistency of Q-CHAT was 0.764 for the overall sample and 0.825 for the hospital sample. Q-CHAT total scores and item scores in the hospital sample remained stable across age groups, indicating age-invariant properties. The hospital sample showed higher endorsement of less favorable development in social interaction and reciprocity items compared to community sample. No difference in the Q-CHAT item scores was present among age groups in the hospital samples. In the community samples, item scores, such as comprehending a child’s speech, using the hand of others as a tool, adapting to a change in routine, repeating the same action, and making unusual finger movements, decreased with the advance of age.

Conclusion

The Korean version of the Q-CHAT demonstrates good validity and reliability and is effective in discriminating autistic traits even in children older than 24 months. The items endorsed for hospital samples varied from community samples, implying item-specific sensitivity for hospital samples.

INTRODUCTION

Autism Spectrum Conditions are a group of neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by persistent difficulties in social interaction and communication, and the presence of repetitive and restricted patterns of interests, behavior, and activities [1]. Early identification of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is important because it allows early intervention, etiologic investigation, and counseling regarding the risk for recurrence [2]. Screening refers to the use of standardized tools at specific intervals to support and refine risk assessment [3]. A screening procedure during regular well-baby checkups has been recommended [4], with the aim of detecting the warning signs of Autism Spectrum Conditions. The process should involve early screening of warning signs and subsequent diagnosis based on clinical judgment, in combination with the application of reliable and standardized gold standard measures [5]. Screening measures suitable for use in young children (i.e., <24 months of age) are available and can be classified as either Level 1 or Level 2 instruments [6].
The Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT) is the most widely used autism screening tool [7], followed by the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) [8] and the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers, Revised with Follow-up (M-CHAT-RF) [9]. All these tools conceptualize ASD-specific traits as categorical variables using a binary scoring system with dichotomous “yes/no” items. This categorical approach has the potential to identify the most obvious signs of ASD among children, and does not identify less evident cases [10]. The Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT), developed by the Autism Research Centre (ARC) of Cambridge University, has been devised to resolve the issues concerning dichotomous items.
The CHAT was originally developed based on behaviors typically exhibited by children around 18 months of age. Its primary purpose is to help physicians identify possible cases of autism, pervasive developmental disorder, and related disorders in children at 18 months [7]. The American Academy of Pediatrics autism expert panel recommends administering the CHAT during preventive care visits for all children at both the 18- and 24-month check-ups [2].
Park et al. [11] conducted a preliminary Korean Q-CHAT validation study with 24 toddlers and preschooler children in a clinical group (2 to 5 years old; mean age, 47.71±13.14 years) and 80 toddlers and children in an unselected group (2 to 4 years old, 46.18±8.57 years). They reported test-retest reliability, internal reliability, and optimal cutoff using a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. In the Korean sample, the optimal cut-off was 33.5, with a sensitivity of 0.75 and specificity of 0.73 [11]. In the selected clinical group, the total Q-CHAT score was 39.1, which was lower than the score of 51.8 reported from the initial study conducted by Allison et al. [12] The Korean version of the Q-CHAT has a lower Cronbach’s alpha (0.83) [11]. The total Q-CHAT score tended to decrease with increasing age [11].
This study aimed to further evaluate the clinical utility of the Korean version of the Q-CHAT in hospital settings by examining the distribution of Q-CHAT scores in both a community sample of toddlers and preschool children and a clinical sample of toddlers and preschool children presenting with developmental delays with a high probability of ASD. Similar to the initial study conducted by Allison et al. [12]. we analyzed toddlers and preschool children aged 12-54 months. Finally, we investigated which Q-CHAT items were more frequently endorsed in the hospital sample, aiming to identify items with greater sensitivity for detecting ASD traits across age groups.

METHODS

Participants

For the community sample, we used Q-CHAT data from a previous Korean Q-CHAT validation study [11] conducted from May 1, 2014 to October 31, 2014. The Q-CHAT was completed by parents of 145 toddlers and preschool children who were recruited from community childcare centers participating in the Infants and Toddlers Early Developmental Screening program of Goyang City’s Child and Adolescent Mental Health Center (CAMHC).
For the hospital sample, we utilized a hospital sample recruited from May 1, 2014, to October 31, 2014, for the previous validation study (n=23) and newly recruited toddlers and children recruited between August 1, 2018 and July 31, 2019 (n=39). The inclusion criteria were children who visited the National Mental Health Medical Center during this period. These children were initially referred to the child and adolescent psychiatry clinic for developmental counseling due to features suggesting a high probability of ASD. Parents of the toddlers and preschool children completed the Q-CHAT, Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), and Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) 1.5-5 by the parents of our participants.
Preliminary demographic analysis revealed a significant difference in the mean age between the community and hospital groups. As a result, we excluded children whose age fell outside of 1 standard deviation (SD) from the mean in both groups. A total of 37 hospital samples and 67 community samples, with ages ranging from 12 to 54 months, were included for the analysis of the reliability and validity of the Korean Q-CHAT [12]. Demographic and socioeconomic information of the participants is also presented in Table 1.

Consent to participate

The study was deemed to present no greater than minimal risk by the Institutional Review Board of the National Center for Mental Health and Dongguk University Ilsan Hospital; hence, a waiver of consent documentation was granted for all the participants who completed assessments before the study began, and a waiver of authorization was granted for all the participants being recruited after the study began.

Ethical approval

This study was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Approval was granted by the ethical committees of the National Center for Mental Health (IRB No.116271-2018-23) and Dongguk University Ilsan Hospital (IRB No.DUIH 2020-06-019).

Measures

Q-CHAT

The Q-CHAT is a questionnaire that measures ASD-specific behavior and characteristics of toddlers. Each question consists of a five-point scale that indicates frequency. The Q-CHAT questions were re-examined by a professor of Korean literature, who revised them to better align with a Korean cultural perspective. The questions were rephrased for clarity, with sentence structure adjusted and the translational tone replaced by more natural, fluent Korean. The Q-CHAT has a total of 25 questions graded on a parent-reported scale, assigning a score of 0 to 4 depending on the frequency of characteristics; the higher the total score, the more likely the child is to receive a diagnosis of ASD. An unresponsive or ambiguous response was treated as zero points as described by Allison et al. [12].

CBCL 1.5-5

The CBCL was developed by Achenbach and Resorla, and the Korean version of the CBCL was standardized in Korea [13]. The K-CBCL 1.5-5 consists of the following two major subscales: 1) problem behavior and 2) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) problem. Problem behaviors include emotional responses, anxiety/depression, physical symptoms, atrophy, attention, aggressive behavior, sleep problems, other problems, and total internalization, and externalization. The DSM diagnostic scale includes emotional problems, anxiety, and behavior. T-scores of the DSM problem subscales of the K-CBCL were used in the analysis.

SCQ (current and lifetime)

The SCQ is a brief 40-item parent-reported screening measure that focuses on items related to ASD symptomatology that are likely to be observed by a primary caregiver. Although the SCQ is a screening tool and thus, cannot be used for the diagnosis of ASD, it is based on the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Two different versions of the SCQ exist. The SCQ Current asks respondents to indicate whether the respective behavior was prevalent during the past three months. In contrast, the SCQ Lifetime references complete developmental history and ask the respondents to indicate whether behaviors have ever been present for questions 2-19 and whether behaviors were present at the age of 4 years or to consider behavior in the past 12 months if the child is not yet 4 years old— for questions 20-40. We used both the SCQ Current and Lifetime questionnaires.

Childhood Autism Rating Scale

The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) is a standardized clinical observational measure designed to distinguish ASDs from other developmental disorders and to distinguish the spectrum of ASD. It is an objective and quantified measure based on behavioral observation instead of subjective clinical judgment and can be used in all age groups, including preschool children. Children’s behavior is evaluated based on a four-point scale in 15 areas (including social association, nonverbal communication, and verbal communication) ranging from age-appropriate to severely abnormal behavior. Higher scores indicate more pronounced ASD characteristics. In this study, if the participants underwent CARS within six months, we used the CARS data obtained from the chart to reduce the burden of preparation of the caregivers’ questionnaire, and we obtained a new CARS score only for participants who did not undergo CARS within six months of the study.

Statistical analysis

All the analyses were performed using SPSS 22.0 (IBM Corp.). Descriptive statistics were used to describe the sex, age, and scores of each screening instrument (Q-CHAT, SCQ-current and lifetime, CBCL, and CARS) for the community and hospital samples. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was calculated to assess the internal consistency of the Q-CHAT. Pearson’s r correlations between the total Q-CHAT score and the screening instruments used in the study were performed to identify the convergent validity of the Q-CHAT. To analyze the optimal cutoff score of the Korean version of the Q-CHAT, the ROC curve was constructed.

RESULTS

Q-CHAT total score distribution for the hospital and community groups across age group

The mean Q-CHAT total score in the hospital sample (mean=42.05, SD=13.62) was significantly higher than in the community sample (mean=29.88, SD=7.83; p<0.001). The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for both the hospital and community groups revealed that the scores were normally distributed (hospital group: K-S[62]=0.097, p>0.01; community group: K-S[144]=0.061, p>0.01). The total score distribution of the Q-CHAT is shown in Supplementary Figure 1. A large percentage of the participants reported Q-CHAT scores ranging from 21 to 40. Overall, the hospital group obtained higher total Q-CHAT scores.
The analysis reported an area under the curve (AUC) of 0.794, which indicated fair diagnostic testing validity. The sensitivity and specificity associated with different cutoff scores for children with ASD-specific traits were calculated. Youden’s index was used to determine the optimal cutoff score for the Korean version of the Q-CHAT. Based on the ROC analysis, the Q-CHAT total score, which better differentiated between children with ASD-specific traits and typically developing children, maximized sensitivity while maintaining an adequated specificity of 32.5 (sensitivity=0.811, specificity=0.687).

Accuracy of the Q-CHAT for predicting ASD and Q-CHAT cut-off under 54 months of age

The Q-CHAT total score, which better differentiated between children with ASD-specific traits and typically developing children, maximized sensitivity while maintaining an adequated specificity was 32.5 (sensitivity=0.811, specificity=0.687) for children aged 12-54 months. The AUC for the Korean version of the Q-CHAT total score for the hospital and community groups under 54 months is shown in Figure 1.

Q-CHAT total scores and SCQ, CARS, and CBCL among participants under 54 months of age

Descriptive statistics of age, total Q-CHAT scores, and mean scores for other ASD screening instruments (SCQ, CARS, and CBCL) among participants under 54 months of age are shown in Table 2. Since the SES (parent education level, occupation, and perceived economic status) of the samples obtained from the CBCL was not significantly different, an independent t-test was performed to confirm the difference between the hospital and community groups and between sex. The mean total Q-CHAT score for all the participants was 31.62 (SD=11.26). Specifically, the hospital sample (M=42.05, SD=13.62) demonstrated a significantly higher Q-CHAT score than the community sample (M=29.88, SD=7.83; t[49.45]=4.99, p<0.001), assuming equal variances. With respect to the CBCL subscale, the hospital sample reported a higher CBCL-pervasive developmental (PD) problem score (M=66.5, SD=9.25) than the community sample (M=54.03, SD=7.41; t[99]=6.83, p<0.001).
The mean score of 40.96 (SD=13.99) for boys in the hospital group was not significantly higher than the mean score of 43.86 (SD=13.30) for girls, while the mean score for boys in the community group (M=33.17, SD=7.79) was significantly higher than the mean score of 26.06 (SD=6.02) for girls. Considering all the ASD screening instruments, including the CARS, SCQ, and CBCL, no significant differences were observed between the boys and girls in the hospital sample (all non-significant [ns]). In contrast, boys (M=55.63, SD=9.03) in the community sample reported greater CBCL-PD problem than girls (M=52.16, SD=4.36; t[52.06]=2.05, p=0.045). This pattern was similar to that of the CBCL-oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) problem, in which boys (M=56.72, SD=8.81) showed higher CBCL-ODD problems than girls (M=52.74, SD=5.73; t[62.73]=2.15, p=0.030).

Convergent validity of the Q-CHAT with the SCQ, CARS, and CBCL under 54 months of age

The correlations among the total Q-CHAT score, age, and other stable screening instruments are shown in Table 3. Age was not significantly correlated with the total Q-CHAT score in the hospital sample, whereas a significant negative correlation was observed in the community sample (r=-0.45, p<0.001).
The total scores of the three screening instruments (SCQ-lifetime, SCQ-current, and CARS) were obtained from the hospital sample. The SCQ-lifetime and -current total scores were positively correlated with the Q-CHAT total scores in the hospital sample (r=0.508 and r=0.565, respectively; both p<0.001). However, no significant correlation was observed between the total Q-CHAT and CARS scores (r=0.278, ns).
The total Q-CHAT scores of the hospital sample were significantly positively correlated with only one t-score from the CBCL’s PD subscales (r=0.387, p<0.01), whereas the total Q-CHAT scores of the community sample were significantly correlated with all the subscales of the CBCL: CBCL-affective (r=0.404, p<0.001), CBCL-anxiety problem (r=0.388, p<0.001), CBCL-PD problem (r=0.470, p<0.001), and CBCL-attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder problem (r=0.337, p<0.001), and CBCL-ODD problem (r=0.568, p<0.001).

Q-CHAT internal consistency and item-total correlations under 54 months of age

The internal consistency of the total Q-CHAT items was good in the overall sample as well as in the hospital sample (Cronbach’s alpha=0.764 and 0.825, respectively). Item removal of lines objects up (item 3) or interest maintained by spinning objects (item 7), resulted in improved internal consistency values of the total items for the hospital sample from Cronbach’s α (0.825 to 0.830 and 0.827, respectively).
Table 4 summarizes the item-total correlations for the hospital and community samples. The Bonferroni correction was used to calculate item-total correlations (p=0.05/25 items=0.002). In the hospital sample, items 5, 6, 8, 9, 10, 12, 15, 17, 19, 23, and 25 were positively correlated with the total Q-CHAT score, and the item-total correlation was good (0.8>r>0.5). Among the correlated items, items 5 (protoimperative pointing), 6 (protodeclarative pointing), 10 (follow the direction of eye gaze), 12 (use hand of others as a tool), 19 (use gestures), and 25 (stare at nothing for long time) were positively correlated with the Q-CHAT total score only for the hospital sample. In the community sample, items 1, 2, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 17, 20, 22, and 23 were positively correlated with the total Q-CHAT score with satisfactory correlation (0.5>r>0.2).
Behaviors, including number of words (item 8), pretend play (item 9), offer comfort to others (item 15), typicality of first words (item 17), and twiddle objects repetitively (item 23), were positively correlated with the Q-CHAT total score in both the samples, and all the items were more strongly correlated with the hospital sample. Behaviors, such as lines objects up (item 3), adapt to change in routine (item 14), repeat exactly what they heard (item 18), check your reaction in unfamiliar situations (item 21), and oversensitive to noise (item 24), were not significantly correlated with the Q-CHAT total score in either sample. Item 18 (repeat exactly what they heard) showed an insignificant but negative correlation with the Q-CHAT total score in both samples (r=-0.299 in the hospital sample and r=-0.066 in the community sample, ns).

Q-CHAT item scored above 2 endorsed for the hospital and community groups

We categorized the ages of the toddlers and children into the following four groups: 1) 12-36 months (n=30), 2) 37-48 months (n=52), and 3) 49-54 months (n=22) (Table 5). The mean ratings of most Q-CHAT items were significantly different between the hospital and community samples of toddlers and children under the age of 54 months, except for items 11, 13, 16, 19, 20, 23, and 24 (ns). Upon comparing the frequency of items scored above 2 in the 12-36 months old (n=30) and 37-48 months old (n=52) groups, the hospital sample more frequently reported the following behaviors: look when call name (item 1), eye contact (item 2), others can comprehend child’s speech (item 4), protoimperative pointing (item 5), protodeclarative pointing (item 6), number of words (item 8), pretend play (item 9), follow the direction of eye gaze (item 10), use of hand of others as a tool (item 12), adapt to change in routine (item 14), offer comfort to others (item 15), typicality of first words (item 17), check your reaction in unfamiliar situations (item 21), stare at nothing for long time (item 25).
However, the ratings of lines up objects (item 3), interest maintained by spinning objects (item 7), sniff/lick unusual object (item11), walk on tiptoes (item 13), repeat same action over and over again (item 16), repeat exactly what they heard (item 18), unusual finger movements (item 20), maintenance of interest in one or two objects (item 22) twiddle objects repetitively (item 23), and oversensitive to noise (item 24) in the community sample were higher or similar to those in the hospital sample.

Q-CHAT item score distribution for the hospital and community groups

The item score distribution of the Q-CHAT for each age group is shown in Supplementary Figure 2. One-way analysis of variance was conducted to compare the effects of age on the Q-CHAT item scores. Significant mean differences were observed among the four age groups in some Q-CHAT items; specifically, in the following items: look when called by name (item 1) (F[3,193]=4.045, p=0.008), eye contact (item 2) (F[3,193]=4.797, p=0.003), others can comprehend child’s speech (item 4) (F[3,193]=13.706, p<0.001), number of words (item 8) (F[3,193]=14.832, p<0.001), follow the direction of eye gaze (item 10) (F[3,192]=3.036, p=0.03), use of hand of others as a tool (item 12) (F[3,193]=6.575, p<0.001), adapt to change in routine (item 14) (F[3,193]=7.360, p<0.001), offer comfort to others (item 15) (F[3,193]=4.279, p=0.006), repeat same action over and over again (item 16) (F[3.193]=5.879, p=0.001), typicality of first words (item 17) (F[3,193]=5.471, p=0.001), and maintenance of interests in one or two objects (item 22) (F[3,193]=3.247, p=0.023).
The effects of age on the Q-CHAT item scores were analyzed separately for the hospital and community samples. No difference in the Q-CHAT item scores was present among age groups in the hospital samples (Table 6), whereas the Q-CHAT item scores of others can comprehend child’s speech (item 4), number of words (item 8), use of hand of others as a tool (item 12), adapt to change in routine (item 14), typicality of first words (item 17), repeat same action over and over again (item 16), unusual finger movements (item 20) and oversensitive to noise (item 24), decreased with advance of age in the community samples (Table 7).

DISCUSSION

This study aimed to further evaluate the clinical utility of the Korean version of the Q-CHAT in hospital settings by examining the distribution of Q-CHAT scores in both a community sample of toddlers and preschool children and a clinical sample of toddlers and preschool children presenting with developmental delays with high probability of ASD. The validity of the Q-CHAT for the risk of ASD was tested through the application of screening instruments, CARS, SCQ, and CBCL, which are commonly used for the evaluation of autism in toddlers. The ROC analysis also demonstrated good screening accuracy. Our result showed that the Korean version of the Q-CHAT had good internal consistency, with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.764. Internal consistency, as reported by Allison et al. [12], showed a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83 in the ASD group (n=160), which is similar to the value found in the present study (0.825, n=68).
Although Q-CHAT was developed to screen for autistic trait in community sample aged 18-24 months, we wanted to test the possibility of Q-CHAT to be used as an initial screening tool for children presenting with developmental delay in hospital settings. In the initial study conducted by Allison et al. [12], the mean age of the ASD group was 44.5 months (SD=10.2 months; range, 19-63 months). In this study we analyzed toddlers and preschool children aged 12-54 months. Similar to previous studies, we observed a normal distribution of the Q-CHAT scores in both groups [14-16], suggesting the unique potential of the Q-CHAT as a dimensional measure of ASD-specific traits along a continuum in the population.
Consistent with the findings reported by Allison et al. [12], children in the hospital sample (M=42.05) scored significantly higher than those in the typically developing community sample (M=29.88). The mean score of the Korean version of the Q-CHAT was 42.05, which was higher than that in a previous Korean study (39.1), but still lower than the mean score of 51.8 from the previous Allison’s study [12]. This could be partly due to a bias resulting from the small sample size of the hospital group. Additionally, the relatively higher mean age of participants (37.5 months) may have contributed to the lower total Q-CHAT score, as previous studies have reported that Q-CHAT scores tend to decrease with increasing age [11,12].
Significant differences in the total Q-CHAT, CBCL-PD problem, and CBCL-ODD. Problem t-scores were observed between male and female toddlers in the community sample. This implies that they follow sex-specific developmental characteristics. In contrast, no sex-specific differences were observed in the scores of other ASD screening instruments in the hospital sample. Since the ASD-specific pathological process was applicable to these children with ASD, we could not find a sex-normal developmental trajectory in the hospital sample. This result also suggests that we do not need to consider applying different cutoff scores according to the toddler’s sex.
Similar to the results of previous study, the Korean version of the Q-CHAT showed good discriminant validity. The total Q-CHAT scores in the hospital group showed significant positive correlations with the PD problem scores on the CARS, SCQ, and CBCL. In contrast, the total Q-CHAT scores in the community sample showed significant positive correlations with not only the PD problem scores of the CBCL but also with all other subscales (affective, anxiety, attention-deficit hyperactivity, and oppositional defiant behavior). This suggests that the Q-CHAT is an effective screening tool for distinguishing PD problems from other affective and behavioral problems in children with ASD.
Finally, we investigated which Q-CHAT items were more frequently endorsed in the hospital sample, aiming to identify items with greater sensitivity for detecting ASD traits across age groups. When the effects of age on the Q-CHAT item scores were analyzed separately for the hospital and community samples, no difference in the Q-CHAT item scores was present among age groups in the hospital samples, whereas the Q-CHAT item scores of others can comprehend child’s speech (item 4), number of words (item 8), use of hand of others as a tool (item 12), adapt to change in routine (item 14), typicality of first words (item 17), repeat same action over and over again (item 16), unusual finger movements (item 20) and oversensitive to noise (item 24), decreased with advance of age in the community samples.
Upon comparing the frequency of items scored above 2 in the 12-36 months old (n=30) and 37-48 months old (n=52) groups, the hospital sample more frequently reported items on social interaction and reciprocity: look when call name (item 1), eye contact (item 2), others can comprehend child’s speech (item 4), protoimperative pointing (item 5), protodeclarative pointing (item 6), number of words (item 8), pretend play (item 9), follow the direction of eye gaze (item 10), use of hand of others as a tool (item 12), adapt to change in routine (item 14), offer comfort to others (item 15), typicality of first words (item 17), check your reaction in unfamiliar situations (item 21), and stare at nothing for long time (item 25).
However, the ratings of repetition and sensory items in the community sample were higher or similar to those in the hospital sample, suggesting that these items were less specific, for example, lines up objects (item 3), interest maintained by spinning objects (item 7), sniff/lick unusual object (item 11), walk on tiptoes (item 13), repeat same action over and over again (item 16), repeat exactly what they heard (item 18), unusual finger movements (item 20), twiddle objects repetitively (item 23), and oversensitive to noise (item 24).
Using the M-CHAT-RF, Guo et al. [17] also noted that certain items may better differentiate patients with ASD from those without ASD. According to Guo et al. [17], the items that are more sensitive to ASD diagnosis were “declarative pointing,” “brings objects to show,” “responses to name,” “gaze following” and “understand what is said,” which were items that assess social interaction and reciprocity. However, “abnormal finger movement” and “hypersensitivity to noise” items, that is, evaluating repetitive/stereotypical behaviors, were not endorsed more frequently by children who were later diagnosed with ASD compared to children who were presumed to not have a diagnosis of ASD [17]. Other previous studies assessing M-CHAT have demonstrated “declarative pointing,” and “brings objects to show” as items with most sensitivity [8,18]. “Response to name” was considered a sensitive item in M-CHAT [8].
The findings of this study suggest that when screening for ASD, social interaction and reciprocity should be assessed more carefully than repetitive and sensory behaviors. Moreover, items endorsed for the hospital sample did not change among age groups, suggesting that the items have similar sensitivity in hospital group indicating stability across age groups. Narvekar et al. [19] tested the effect of atypicality of responsiveness on autism trait, which included both under-responsiveness and hyper-responsiveness in infants to external stimuli. They found that in infants with a high risk of autism, hyperreactivity at 14 months was positively associated with fear at 24 months, and hyper-reactivity at 24 months was longitudinally associated with restricted and repetitive behavior and deference in social interaction at 36 months. When the analysis of under-responsiveness was added to the analysis of hyper-responsiveness, the longitudinal association between hyper-responsiveness, restricted and repetitive behavior and social interaction and reciprocity became statistically insignificant, suggesting that various types of reactivity must be considered to understand the developmental path of autism and item specificity.
This study has certain limitations that should be acknowledged. First, the study had an unequal proportion of toddlers and children in the two sample groups, with the hospital group comprising half the sample size of the typically developing (community) group. Second, children in the hospital sample were significantly younger than those in the other groups. Replication with a larger and better age-matched sample of children with ASD and typically developing children is recommended. Third, the hospital group participated in the study prior to receiving an autism diagnosis. However, parents may have been influenced by their recognition of their child’s delays in language, social development, and other developmental concerns, as well as the fact that they had already made appointments with specialists. Finally, although most participants in the hospital sample showed a high probability of ASD, the sample was not divided into groups based on subsequent diagnoses. As a result, it was not possible to compare the Q-CHAT scores with longitudinal diagnostic outcomes.
In conclusion, the Korean version of the Q-CHAT demonstrates good validity and reliability and is effective in discriminating autistic traits even in children older than 24 months. This supports its utility as an initial screening tool for autistic traits in Korean clinical settings. Moreover, the items endorsed for hospital samples varied from those endorsed for community samples, implying item-specific sensitivity for hospital samples.

Supplementary Materials

The Supplement is available with this article at https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2024.0116.
Supplementary Figure 1.
Total score range of the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT) Korean version. Under54_hospital: hospital group participants who is under 54 months of age. Under54_community: community group participants who is over 54 months of age. Over54_hospital: hospital group participants who is 54 months of age or more. Over54_community: community group participants who is over 54 months of age or more.
pi-2024-0116-Supplementary-Fig-1.pdf
Supplementary Figure 2.
Mean score of each item in the Korean version of Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers according to age group.
pi-2024-0116-Supplementary-Fig-2.pdf

Notes

Availability of Data and Material

The datasets generated or analyzed during the study are not publicly available due to the restriction by Institutional Review Board.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Yeni Kim. Data curation: Namhee Yoo, Jungwon Choi, Yeni Kim. Formal analysis: Namhee Yoo, Jungwon Choi, Yeni Kim. Funding acquisition: Yeni Kim. Investigation: Namhee Yoo, Youngeun Hong, Kyungjin Lee, Yeni Kim. Methodology: Yeni Kim. Project administration: Yeni Kim. Resources: Yeni Kim. Software: Youngeun Hong. Supervision: Yeni Kim. Validation: Youngeun Hong. Visualization: Youngeun Hong, Kyungjin Lee. Writing—original draft: Youngeun Hong, Kyungjin Lee, Yeni Kim. Writing—review & editing: Kyungjin Lee, Yeni Kim.

Funding Statement

This research was supported by a grant from the Korea Health Technology R&D Project through the Korea Health Industry Development Institute (KHIDI) (grant number HI14C1098).

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors are grateful to all the children and their families who participated in this study and also Prof. Baron-Cohen of Autism Research Center for giving us permission to translate and use the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers.

Figure 1.
ROC curve for the Korean version of Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers under 54 months of age. ROC, receiver operating characteristic.
pi-2024-0116f1.jpg
Table 1.
Descriptive characteristics of the total participants (total N=207)
Characteristics Hospital (N=62) Community (N=145) Selected data (under 54 mon)
Hospital* Community
Age (mon)
 12-24 2 2 2 2
 25-36 16 10 16 10
 37-48 14 38 14 38
 49-54 5 17 5 17
 55-84 17 77 - -
Sex
 Male 43 77 23 36
 Female 18 67 14 31
Socioeconomic status
 Upper 0 1 0 0
 Middle 44 111 28 51
 Lower 3 8 2 5
Education of the mother
 High school 10 35 8 13
 Undergraduate 35 90 20 47
 Graduate 6 7 6 3
Occupation of the mother
 Skilled workers 2 12 2 6
 Self-employed or sales 0 6 0 2
 Clerical or white-collar 8 23 7 17
 Management or quasi-professional 2 2 2 2
 Professional 4 9 2 4
 Artist or clergy 0 9 0 3
 Housewife or unemployed 34 64 20 26
Education of the father
 High school 8 10 5 14
 Undergraduate 35 89 23 42
 Graduate 8 31 6 8
Occupation of the father
 Unskilled workers 4 2 2 0
 Skilled workers 9 22 7 9
 Self-employed or sales 8 35 5 15
 Clerical or white-collar 19 40 11 21
 Management or quasi-professional 6 20 5 10
 Professional 3 6 2 4
 Artist or clergy 0 5 0 2
 House-work or unemployed 1 1 1 1

* hospital group is selected data (N=37) from total hospital sample recruited;

community group is selected data (N=67) from total community sample recruited.

-, not available

Table 2.
Descriptive statistics of Q-CHAT and other screening instruments (SCQ, CARS, and CBCL) for children younger than 54 months of age
Hospital (N=37) Community (N=67) t Hospital group
Community group
Male (N=23) Female (N=14) t Male (N=36) Female (N=31) t
Age (mon) 37.49±8.28 43.06±7.79 3.42** 39.22±8.08 34.64±8.08 1.67 42.08±8.78 44.19±6.42 -1.11
Q-CHAT 42.05±13.62 29.88±7.83 4.99** 40.96±13.99 43.86±13.30 -0.62 33.17±7.79 26.06±6.02 4.13**
CBCL-affective problem 55.15±7.60 53.88±6.63 0.867 55.68±8.62 54.16±5.46 0.55 54.47±7.17 53.19±6.00 0.78
Anxiety problem 54.88±7.58 53.51±5.87 1.01 56.41±8.35 52.86±4.60 0.59 54.33±6.46 52.55±5.03 1.25
PD problem 66.50±9.25 54.03±7.41 6.83** 66.14±9.91 67.17±8.27 -0.31 55.63±9.03 52.16±4.36 2.05
ADHD problem 58.56±7.92 55.06±7.02 2.27 58.73±9.16 58.25±5.26 0.16 55.81±7.58 54.19±6.32 0.94
ODD problem 57.94±8.57 54.88±7.75 1.81 58.32±9.87 57.25±5.79 0.34 56.72±8.81 52.74±5.73 2.15
CARS - - - 25.09±6.15 21.57±8.26 1.48 - - -
SCQ-current, - - - 13.00±5.86 15.31±8.07 -0.98 - - -
lifetime 13.35±5.9 14.00±7.638 -0.29

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation unless otherwise indicated.

** p<0.001.

Q-CHAT, Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; PD, pervasive developmental; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; -, not avaiable

Table 3.
Pearson correlations among ASD screening instruments with Q-CHAT for children younger than 54 months of age
Q-CHAT total score Total participants Hospital group Community group
1. Age -0.435** -0.241 -0.450**
2. SCQ-lifetime 0.508** -
3. SCQ-current 0.565** -
4. CARS 0.278 -
5. CBCL-affective problem 0.334** 0.256 0.404**
6. CBCL-anxiety problem 0.294** 0.17 0.388**
7. CBCL-PD problem 0.588** 0.387* 0.470**
8. CBCL-ADHD problem 0.271** 0.018 0.337**
9. CBCL-ODD problem 0.391** 0.091 0.568**

* p<0.01;

** p<0.001.

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; Q-CHAT, Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; PD, pervasive developmental; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; -, not available

Table 4.
Item-total correlation of the Q-CHAT for children younger than 54 months of age
Item Hospital sample Community sample
1. Look when called by name 0.405 0.399**
2. Eye contact 0.351 0.499**
3. Lines up objects 0.176 0.072
4. Others can comprehend child’s speech 0.502 0.514**
5. Protoimperative pointing 0.687** 0.05
6. Protodeclarative pointing 0.725** 0.29
7. Interest maintained by spinning objects 0.219 0.424**
8. Number of words 0.673** 0.537**
9. Pretend play 0.562** 0.367
10. Follow the direction of eye gaze 0.536** 0.342
11. Sniff/lick unusual objects 0.359 0.404**
12. Use hand of others as a tool 0.546** 0.195
13. Walk on tiptoes 0.4 0.418**
14. Adapt to change in routine 0.416 0.159
15. Offer comfort to others 0.523** 0.413**
16. Repeat same action 0.433 0.503**
17. Typicality of first words 0.612** 0.476**
18. Repeat exactly what they heard -0.299 -0.066
19. Use gestures 0.679** 0.214
20. Unusual finger movements 0.231 0.455**
21. Check your reaction in unfamiliar situations 0.273 -0.023
22. Maintenance of interest in one or two objects 0.358 0.432**
23. Twiddle objects repetitively 0.556** 0.466**
24. Oversensitive to noise 0.392 0.363
25. Stare at nothing for long time 0.561** 0.247

** p<0.001;

refers to the reverse question; we used recoded value in all analyses.

Q-CHAT, Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers

Table 5.
Frequency of each items scored above 2 according to age group
Item 12-36 months (N=30)
37-48 months (N=52)
49-54 months (N=22)
H (N=18) C (N=12) H (N=14) C (N=38) H (N=5) C (N=17)
1. Look when called by name 11 (61.1) 2 (16.7) 6 (42.9) 0 1 (20.0) 1 (5.9)
2. Eye contact 8 (44.4) 1 (8.3) 8 (57.1) 0 2 (40.0) 1 (5.9)
3. Lines up objects 14 (77.8) 10 (83.3) 11 (78.6) 36 (94.7) 3 (60.0) 17 (100)
4. Comprehend child’s speech 16 (88.9) 4 (33.3) 11 (78.6) 4 (10.5) 2 (40.0) 0
5. Protoimperative pointing 6 (33.3) 2 (16.7) 5 (35.7) 6 (15.8) 1 (20.0) 6 (35.3)
6. Protodeclarative pointing 9 (50.0) 4 (33.3) 6 (42.9) 4 (10.5) 1 (20.0) 6 (35.3)
7. Interest maintained by spinning objects 4 (22.2) 6 (50.0) 2 (14.3) 20 (52.6) 2 (40.0) 10 (58.8)
8. Number of words 14 (77.8) 5 (41.7) 7 (50.0) 3 (7.9) 3 (60.0) 1 (5.9)
9. Pretend play 9 (50.0) 4 (33.3) 5 (35.7) 12 (31.6) 3 (60.0) 2 (11.8)
10. Follow the eye gaze 7 (38.9) 1 (8.3) 4 (28.6) 4 (10.5) 3 (60.0) 3 (17.6)
11. Sniff/lick unusual objects 10 (55.6) 7 (58.3) 6 (42.9) 23 (60.5) 2 (40.0) 5 (29.4)
12. Use hand of others as a tool 15 (83.3) 12 (100) 14 (100) 28 (73.7) 4 (80.0) 9 (52.9)
13. Walk on tiptoes 9 (50.0) 8 (66.7) 4 (28.6) 20 (52.6) 5 (100) 5 (29.4)
14. Adapt to change in routine 5 (27.8) 2 (16.7) 6 (42.9) 2 (5.3) 2 (40.0) 0
15. Offer comfort to others 12 (66.7) 5 (41.7) 9 (64.3) 2 (5.3) 4 (80.0) 2 (11.8)
16. Repeat same action 15 (83.3) 9 (75.0) 8 (57.1) 22 (57.9) 4 (80.0) 7 (41.2)
17. Typicality of first words 12 (66.7) 3 (25.0) 3 (21.4) 6 (15.8) 2 (40.0) 1 (5.9)
18. Repeat what they heard 12 (66.7) 9 (75.0) 13 (92.9) 37 (97.4) 3 (60.0) 16 (94.1)
19. Use gestures 4 (22.2) 1 (8.3) 2 (14.3) 5 (13.2) 0 3 (17.6)
20. Unusual finger movements 4 (22.2) 4 (33.3) 2 (14.3) 3 (7.9) 2 (40.0) 1 (5.9)
21. Check your reaction 10 (55.6) 3 (25.0) 8 (57.1) 11 (28.9) 2 (40.0) 10 (58.8)
22. Maintenance of interest in one or two objects 7 (38.9) 7 (58.3) 4 (28.6) 18 (47.4) 2 (40.0) 10 (58.8)
23. Twiddle objects repetitively 8 (44.4) 7 (58.3) 7 (50) 21 (55.3) 2 (40.0) 8 (47.1)
24. Oversensitive to noise 10 (55.6) 9 (75.0) 9 (64.3) 28 (73.7) 3 (60.0) 6 (35.3)
25. Stare at nothing for long time 10 (55.6) 1 (8.3) 6 (42.9) 3 (7.9) 2 (40.0) 1 (5.9)

Values are presented as number (%). H, hospital group; C, community group

Table 6.
One-way analysis of variance for the effects of age on the Q-CHAT item scores in Hospital samples
Item 12-36 months (N=18) 37-48 months (N=14) 49-54 months (N=5) F p
1. Look when called by name 1.83±0.99 1.36±0.84 1.00±0.71 2.132 0.134
2. Eye contact 1.56±0.86 1.64±0.84 1.40±0.55 0.165 0.848
3. Lines up objects 2.56±1.04 1.93±1.21 2.00±1.58 1.234 0.304
4. Comprehend child’s speech 3.17±1.15 2.50±1.29 2.00±1.41 2.227 0.123
5. Protoimperative pointing 1.56±1.76 1.14±1.17 0.60±0.90 0.913 0.411
6. Protodeclarative pointing 1.94±1.70 1.64±1.22 1.00±1.23 0.819 0.450
7. Interest maintained by spinning objects 0.83±0.92 0.86±1.03 1.20±1.79 0.229 0.796
8. Number of words 2.61±1.54 1.57±1.09 1.60±1.68 2.503 0.097
9. Pretend play 2.00±1.57 1.64±1.34 2.00±1.58 0.251 0.780
10. Follow the eye gaze 1.56±1.29 1.21±1.05 1.80±0.84 0.592 0.559
11. Sniff/lick unusual objects 1.56±1.29 1.07±1.27 1.40±1.95 0.490 0.617
12. Use hand of others as a tool 3.22±1.35 3.21±0.89 3.20±1.79 0.001 0.999
13. Walk on tiptoes 1.56±1.10 1.00±0.78 2.80±0.45 6.975 0.003
14. Adapt to change in routine 1.11±0.68 1.14±0.86 1.00±1.00 0.060 0.942
15. Offer comfort to others 2.39±1.50 1.86±0.77 2.40±1.52 0.765 0.473
16. Repeat same action 2.56±1.10 1.57±0.94 2.60±1.67 3.395 0.045
17. Typicality of first words 2.28±1.78 1.14±1.35 1.60±1.82 1.932 0.161
18. Repeat what they heard 2.17±1.51 3.00±0.96 1.80±1.79 2.090 0.139
19. Use gestures 1.06±1.43 0.57±0.76 0.80±0.45 0.730 0.489
20. Unusual finger movements 0.67±1.24 0.57±1.09 0.80±1.10 0.075 0.928
21. Check your reaction 1.61±0.92 1.86±1.10 1.20±0.84 0.850 0.436
22. Maintenance of interest in one or two objects 1.06±1.00 0.93±0.83 2.20±1.64 2.945 0.066
23. Twiddle objects repetitively 1.50±1.65 1.50±1.29 1.40±1.52 0.010 0.991
24. Oversensitive to noise 1.72±1.36 1.86±1.17 1.80±1.30 0.044 0.957
25. Stare at nothing for long time 1.56±1.34 1.14±1.03 1.20±1.79 0.440 0.648

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation unless otherwise indicated.

refers to the reverse question; we used recoded value in all analyses.

Q-CHAT, Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers

Table 7.
One-way analysis of variance for the effects of age on the Q-CHAT item scores in community samples
Item 12-36 months (N=12) 37-48 months (N=38) 49-54 months (N=17) F p
1. Look when called by name 0.50±0.80 0.11±0.31 0.41±0.80 3.08 0.053
2. Eye contact 0.67±0.65 0.26±0.45 0.35±0.61 2.65 0.078
3. Lines up objects 2.42±1.31 2.79±.91 2.71±0.77 0.69 0.505
4. Comprehend child’s speech 1.42±1.17 0.55±0.76 0.35±0.49 7.09 0.002
5. Protoimperative pointing 0.67±1.23 0.53±0.76 1.00±1.23 1.36 0.265
6. Protodeclarative pointing 1.33±1.67 0.45±0.76 1.06±0.97 4.38 0.016
7. Interest maintained by spinning objects 1.75±1.06 1.63±1.20 1.59±0.87 0.08 0.923
8. Number of words 1.33±1.07 0.34±0.63 0.35±0.61 9.30 <0.001
9. Pretend play 1.08±1.38 0.79±0.91 0.59±0.71 0.93 0.400
10. Follow the eye gaze 0.67±1.16 0.50±0.69 0.82±1.07 0.81 0.452
11. Sniff/lick unusual objects 2.00±1.76 1.84±1.26 1.12±1.41 1.95 0.151
12. Use hand of others as a tool 3.50±0.67 2.53±1.27 1.71±1.65 6.74 0.002
13. Walk on tiptoes 1.75±0.62 1.42±0.83 1.00±1.12 2.69 0.076
14. Adapt to change in routine 1.00±0.60 0.87±0.46 0.53±0.51 3.67 0.031
15. Offer comfort to others 1.17±1.34 0.55±0.60 0.59±1.06 2.25 0.114
16. Repeat same action 2.50±1.62 2.21±1.47 1.24±1.15 3.59 0.033
17. Typicality of first words 1.33±0.99 0.82±0.93 0.41±0.62 3.95 0.024
18. Repeat what they heard 2.83±1.59 3.29±0.90 2.82±0.95 1.56 0.218
19. Use gestures 0.33±0.65 0.53±0.80 0.76±0.75 1.18 0.315
20. Unusual finger movements 1.08±1.56 0.37±0.71 0.18±0.53 4.04 0.022
21. Check your reaction 0.92±0.79 1.16±0.82 1.71±1.16 3.10 0.052
22. Maintenance of interest in one or two objects 1.75±1.06 1.55±1.01 1.71±0.99 0.24 0.784
23. Twiddle objects repetitively 1.75±1.55 1.76±1.26 1.35±1.00 0.67 0.517
24. Oversensitive to noise 2.17±1.12 2.24±0.97 1.47±0.94 3.66 0.031
25. Stare at nothing for long time 0.33±0.89 0.47±0.65 0.29±0.59 0.49 0.618

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation unless otherwise indicated.

refers to the reverse question; we used recoded value in all analyses.

Q-CHAT, Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers

REFERENCES

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed). Arlington: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.

2. Johnson CP, Myers SM, American Academy of Pediatrics Council on Children with Disabilities. Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2007;120:1183-1215.
crossref pmid pdf
3. Council on Children with Disabilities; Section on Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics; Bright Futures Steering Committee; Medical Home Initiatives for Children with Special Needs Project Advisory Committee. Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: an algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics 2006;118:405-420.
crossref pmid pdf
4. Filipek PA, Accardo PJ, Ashwal S, Baranek GT, Cook EH Jr, Dawson G, et al. Practice parameter: screening and diagnosis of autism: report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurology Society. Neurology 2000;55:468-479.
crossref pmid
5. Petrocchi S, Levante A, Lecciso F. Systematic review of level 1 and level 2 screening tools for autism spectrum disorders in toddlers. Brain Sci 2020;10:180
crossref pmid pmc
6. Robins DL, Dumont-Mathieu TM. Early screening for autism spectrum disorders: update on the modified checklist for autism in toddlers and other measures. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2006;27(2 Suppl):S111-S119.
pmid
7. Baron-Cohen S, Allen J, Gillberg C. Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. Br J Psychiatry 1992;161:839-843.
crossref pmid
8. Robins DL, Fein D, Barton ML, Green JA. The modified checklist for autism in toddlers: an initial study investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31:131-144.
pmid
9. Robins DL, Casagrande K, Barton M, Chen CM, Dumont-Mathieu T, Fein D. Validation of the modified checklist for autism in toddlers, revised with follow-up (M-CHAT-R/F). Pediatrics 2014;133:37-45.
crossref pmid pmc
10. Ruta L, Chiarotti F, Arduino GM, Apicella F, Leonardi E, Maggio R, et al. Validation of the quantitative checklist for autism in toddlers in an Italian clinical sample of young children with autism and other developmental disorders. Front Psychiatry 2019;10:488
crossref pmid pmc
11. Park S, Won E, Lee JH, Yoon S, Park EJ, Kim Y. Reliability and validity of the Korean translation of quantitative checklist for autism in toddlers: a preliminary study. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2018;29:80-85.
crossref
12. Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Charman T, Richler J, Pasco G, et al. The Q-CHAT (quantitative checklist for autism in toddlers): a normally distributed quantitative measure of autistic traits at 18-24 months of age: preliminary report. J Autism Dev Disord 2008;38:1414-1425.
crossref pmid pdf
13. Kim YA, Lee J, Moon SJ, Kim YJ, Oh KJ. [Standardization study for the Korean version of the child behavior checklist for ages 1.5-5]. Kor J Clin Psychol 2009;28:117-136. Korean.
crossref
14. Auyeung B, Taylor K, Hackett G, Baron-Cohen S. Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children. Mol Autism 2010;1:11
crossref pmid pmc
15. Magiati I, Goh DA, Lim SJ, Gan DZ, Leong JC, Allison C, et al. The psychometric properties of the quantitative-checklist for autism in toddlers (Q-CHAT) as a measure of autistic traits in a community sample of Singaporean infants and toddlers. Mol Autism 2015;6:40
pmid pmc
16. Wong HS, Huertas-Ceballos A, Cowan FM, Modi N, Medicines for Neonates Investigator Group. The psychometric properties of the quantitative-checklist for autism in toddlers (Q-CHAT) as a measure of autistic traits in a community sample of Singaporean infants and toddlers. J Pediatr 2014;164:26-33.e1.
pmid
17. Guo C, Luo M, Wang X, Huang S, Meng Z, Shao J, et al. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of modified checklist for autism in toddlers, revised, with follow-up (M-CHAT-R/F). J Autism Dev Disord 2019;49:185-196.
crossref pmid pdf
18. Wong V, Hui LH, Lee WC, Leung LS, Ho PK, Lau WL, et al. A modified screening tool for autism (checklist for autism in toddlers [CHAT-23]) for Chinese children. Pediatrics 2004;114:e166-e176.
crossref pmid pdf
19. Narvekar N, Carter Leno V, Pasco G, Begum Ali J, Johnson MH, Charman T, et al. The roles of sensory hyperreactivity and hyporeactivity in understanding infant fearfulness and emerging autistic traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2024;65:1022-1036.
crossref pmid


ABOUT
AUTHOR INFORMATION
ARTICLE CATEGORY

Browse all articles >

BROWSE ARTICLES
Editorial Office
#522, 27, Seochojungang-ro 24-gil, Seocho-gu, Seoul 06601, Korea
Tel: +82-2-717-5543    E-mail: psychiatryinvest@gmail.com                

Copyright © 2025 by Korean Neuropsychiatric Association.

Developed in M2PI

Close layer
prev next