Are Tattoos an Indicator of Severity of Non-Suicidal Self-Injury Behavior in Adolescents?

Article information

Psychiatry Investig. 2019;16(7):504-512
Publication date (electronic) : 2019 July 25
doi : https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2019.03.06
1Hospital Psiquiátrico Infantil “Dr. Juan N. Navarro”, Mexico City, Mexico
2Clinical and Community Epidemiology in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
3Hospital Psiquiátrico “Dr. Samuel Ramírez Moreno”, Mexico State, Mexico
4Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatría “Dr. Ramón de la Fuente Muñiz”, Mexico City, Mexico
5Hospital General “Dr. Ruben Leñero”, Mexico City, Mexico
6CECOSAM Cuauhtémoc, Mexico City, Mexico
7Clinical and Community Epidemiology in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
Correspondence: Lilia Albores-Gallo, MD, PhD Departamento de Epidemiología Genética, Clínica y Comunitaria; Hospital Psiquiátrico Infantil “Dr. Juan N. Navarro” San Buenaventura 86, Belisario Domínguez, Tlalpan 14080, Ciudad de México, México Tel: +525523162517, Fax: +5255739161, E-mail: lilialbores@gmail.com
Received 2018 October 24; Revised 2018 December 17; Accepted 2019 March 6.

Abstract

Objective

To compare adolescents with non-suicidal self-injury behavior and tattoos [NSSI (T+)] with another group with non-suicidal self-injury behavior without tattoos [NSSI (T-)].

Methods

Adolescents (n=438) 42.6% males from the community (M=12.3, SD=1.3), completed the Self-Injury Schedule.

Results

The lifetime prevalence of tattoos performed with the purpose to feel pain was 1.8%. Compared to the NSSI (T-) group, the NSSI (T+) group was significantly more likely to meet the DSM-5 frequency criteria of 5 self-injury events in 1 year, practice more than one method of self-injury, and topography, more suicidal intentionality, more negative thoughts and affective emotions before, during, and after self-injury and more academic and social dysfunction.

Conclusion

Adolescents from the community who practice tattooing to feel pain, show a distinct phenotype of NSSI. Health professionals and pediatricians should assess tattooing characteristics such as intention (to feel pain), frequency, and presence of non-suicidal self-injury behavior and suicide intentionality.

INTRODUCTION

Prevalence of tattooing in the general population it is between 6% to 24% [1], in clinical samples of adolescents it is between 4.5% and 13.2% [2-5], the highest figures are reported in young adults, and militia-related individuals [5,6].

Recent studies show an association between tattooing and psychiatric disorders, such as depression [7] anxiety, eating disorders [5] and risky behaviors like substance use [5,8-12], promiscuity [11,13], suicidal thinking and attempted suicide [5,7,9,14,15].

Adult tattoo users report self-injury behavior more frequently [16,17], sharing psychological precipitants such as negative thoughts of anxiety, tension, anger, distress or depression and interpersonal difficulties before the act [16,18]. These association has been poorly studied in adolescents, although, research suggests those who acquire permanent tattoos during adolescence, specially at earlier ages (11–16 years), have more risk factors for psychopathology and other negative outcomes and engage more frequently in risky behaviors [4,5,9,19,20].

Tattoos and piercings have been considered as methods of self-harm, but, they may have social acceptance in some cultures [21,22]; this explains why under the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edition (DSM-5) [23], tattoos were excluded from the non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) condition, specifically mentioned in criterion D: “Behavior (self-injury) is not socially accepted (as an example, piercings, tattoos, part of a religious or cultural ritual...).”

The aim of this study was to compare the characteristics of NSSI behavior with and without tattoos performed with the intention to feel pain in a sample of Mexican adolescents.

The Research and Ethics Committee of the participating institutions approved the study (II1/01/1013).

Hypothesis

If the presence of tattoos is associated with risky behaviors in adolescents, then, adolescents who tattoo themselves with the intention to feel pain and practice NSSI will injure more often, in more anatomical sites, experience more negative feelings and cognitive states and greater dysfunction.

METHODS

A survey with a cross-sectional and comparative design was conducted involving adolescents (n=438) between 11 and 17 years of age, with a mean age of 12.3 years (SD 1.3), 57.2% were female (n=251).

Instruments

The Non-Suicide Self-Injury Schedule (NSSI-S) [24]

The instrument is based on the DSM-5 criteria for NSSI, it contains 31 dichotomous items with a yes/no response format to explore the DSM-5 dimensions of self-injuring: B1: Psychological precipitants, B2: Concerns, B3: Urgencies, B4: Contingent responses, C1: Interpersonal difficulties, C2: Preoccupation with the behavior, C3: Frequent thoughts about self-injuring, E: Impairment and some explanatory variables. The frequency of self-injuring according to DSM-5 Criteria; the methods and topography; the emotions present before, during and after self-injuring; the associated addictive component; the triggering and mitigating factors for self-injuring; the suicidal intentionality and the age of onset of the behavior.

Cases were assigned to two groups if they gave a positive answer to item 1: “Do you hurt yourself without the intention to die?” and answered yes/no to item 31: “Did you ever get a tattoo to feel pain?” Group 1) students with non-suicidal self-injury and tattoos with the intention to feel pain [NSSI (T+)] and group 2) students with non-suicidal self-injury without tattoos [NSSI (T-)].

Procedure

After obtaining approval from administrators of two public high schools and parents, those who agreed to participate signed an informed consent. Students gave their assent and completed the instrument in their classrooms. All participants received information on how to seek attention in our institution.

Statistical analysis

Categorical demographic variables were analyzed using frequencies and percentages, and continuous variables were analyzed using the Student t-test. The chi-square or Fisher test was used for categorical variables and unadjusted odds ratios were calculated to measure the effect size of the association between practicing non-suicidal self-injury behavior and tattoos to feel pain (yes/no) and the rest of the variables of the NSSI-S. A statistical significance level of p=0.05 was established.

An algorithm was used to perform the statistical analysis used in this study as follows: Dimension A explores the methods used for self-injury through items 5–16. The higher the score, the more serious the self-harmful behavior. The 12 items were added, and a new binary variable was created recoding to 0= those who do not injure themselves and 1= those who injured themselves with one or more methods. Items 2–4 inquire about frequency according to DSM-5 criteria.

Dimension B evaluates the contingent response through items 17–22, which were added, and a new binary variable was recoded assigning to 0 those with no feelings or cognitive states and 1 to those presenting one or more negative feeling or cognition.

Dimension C explores if the behavior is associated with at least one of the following: interpersonal difficulties or negative feelings or thoughts.

Criterion D was excluded for the purpose of this research.

Dimension E assesses social, school and daily living impairment in the school area, other activities of daily living (resting, sleeping) and in the social environment, through items 23–25; the items were added, and a new binary variable was recoded assigning a 0 to those who had no social, school and daily living dysfunction and 1 to those who had more than one.

RESULTS

The prevalence of NSSI was 11.5% (n=50), while 1.8% (n=8) reported having a tattoo performed with the intention to feel pain. The frequency of tattooing was larger in males compared to females (2.2% vs. 1.6%), but this difference did not reach statistical significance.

The presence of NSSI behavior was larger in students with tattoos (62.5%) than in adolescents without them (10.6%) (OR:14; 95% CI: 3.2–60.5; p=0.001), Figure 1 shows a comparison of the number of self-injuring events per month, 6 months and 12 months for each group.

Figure 1.

Frequency of events by group.

The DSM-5 frequency criterion (5 events per year) was higher in the NSSI (T+) group compared to the NSSI (T-), 75% vs. 7.7% respectively (OR: 35.7; 95% CI: 6.9–184); in the last month, 62.5% vs. 6.1% (OR: 25.5; 95% CI: 5.7–112.9) and in the previous 6 months, 50% vs 9.4% (OR: 9.6; 95% CI: 2.3–40) (Figure 1).

Table 1 shows the results of comparing the NSSI (T-) and NSSI (T+) groups according to the method and topography of self-injury.

Characteristics of NSSI

Method of self-injury

The NSSI (T+) group was significantly more likely to practice all methods of self-injury, with the top three including sticking objects under the skin (OR: 29; 95% CI: 6.5–129.1), using objects to mark the skin or nails (OR: 17; 95% CI: 3.9–73.9) and rubbing the skin (OR: 13.4; 95% CI: 3.1–58%).

Topography

The NSSI (T+) group was significantly more likely to selfinjure in buttocks and/or genitals (OR: 252.6; 95% CI: 40.6–1570.1), face (OR: 87.8; 95% CI: 16.2–474.5), head (OR: 29.4; 95%CI: 6.6–129.8) and arms and elbows (OR: 26.9; 95% CI: 6.1–118.1).

DSM-5 criteria

Psychological precipitants

The results of the comparison between the NSSI (T-) and NSSI (T+) groups on psychological precipitants are reported in Table 2. Compared to the NSSI (T-) group, the NSSI (T+) group was significantly more likely to have negative emotional and cognitive states before, during, and after the selfinjury. The effect size was higher for feelings of anxiety (OR: 40.1; 95% CI: 7.4–217.0), self-criticism (OR: 38.2; 95% CI: 7.4–197.3) and tension (OR: 29.2; 95% CI: 6.5–129.8) before the behavior.

Psychological precipitants of NSSI

Addictive component

The NSSI (T+) group reported more addictive features, like feelings of addiction to self-injurious behavior (OR: 55.1; 95% CI: 10.0–303.7), being unable to stop doing the self-injurious act (OR: 8.7; 95% CI: 2.1–36.3) and desire to stop doing the self-injurious act (OR: 7.2; 95% CI: 1.5–33.1) (Table 3).

Addictive component of NSSI

Table 4 shows the groups comparison between DSM-5 criteria for Dimension A, Dimension B (B1, B2 y B3), Dimension C (C1, C2, C3) and Dimension E.

NSSI dimensions

Dimension A

Compared to the NSSI (T-) group, the NSSI (T+) self-injured more frequently (Dimension A) (OR: 7.9; 95% CI: 1.0–64.8).

Dimension B

The NSSI (T+) reported a larger percentage of contingent responses to self-jury (Dimension B), such as: Releasing negative feelings (B1) (OR: 15.2; 95% CI: 3.5–66), Resolving interpersonal difficulties (B2) (OR: 14.1; 95% CI: 3.3–59.5) and Induction of positive feelings (B3) (OR: 19; 95% CI: 4.1–88.1).

Dimension C

The NSSI (T+), had a larger percentage of negative emotions and cognitive states associated with NSSI, including interpersonal difficulties, negatives feelings or thoughts (OR: 33.8; 95% CI: 4.1–279.3), and preoccupation with the intended behavior that is difficult to control (OR:11.1, 95% CI: 2.6–46.3) (Tables 2 and 4).

Dimension E

The NSSI (T+), had more impairment (Dimension E) on Academic activities (OR: 95.3, 95% CI: 17.5–517.9), Interpersonal relationships (OR: 22.3; 95% CI: 4.7–105.4), and Rest and sleep (OR: 29.2; 95% CI: 6.6–128.9).

Suicidal intention

The NSSI (T+) group was more likely to self-injure without suicidal intent, 62.5% compared to 10.6% of the NSSI (T-) but also reported more suicidal thoughts during the self-injurious behavior, 50% vs 4.9% (OR: 19.2; 95% CI: 4.4–82.3). The majority of the NSSI (T+) had more suicidal intent (87.5%) when self-injuring compared to only 2.1% of the NSSI (T-) (OR: 323.5; 95% CI: 35.9–2911.0) (Table 5).

Suicidal Intent

DISCUSSION

This study demonstrated differences in the characteristics of self-injury behavior in two groups of adolescents who practice self-harm with and without tattoos: NSSI (T+) and NSSI (T-). The lack of similar studies makes it hard to compare our results.

In our study, the frequency of tattoos with the intention to feel pain in the total sample was 1.8% which is similar to adolescents of the community from Chile (1.7%) and Taiwan (1%) [25,26]. Turkey and Canada reported a higher prevalence (4.8% and 7.9%) [4,10], but these studies assessed how frequently the participants got tattoos but not the motivating factors for getting tattooed.

In our study, the prevalence of tattoos performed to feel pain in adolescents with NSSI in the community was higher (10%); this figure approximates the prevalence in clinical samples, however, most of the studies included adults [2,3,27].

In our study, the frequency of tattoos by sex (male 2.2% vs. girls 1.6%) showed no significant differences, this result is consistent with other studies in the community and in clinical samples [4,10,17,20]. In contrast, other research shows increased frequency in females [9,11,28-32].

DSM-5 criteria

Dimension A

The NSSI (T+) group met the DSM-5 frequency criteria of 5 events per year proposed by Shaffer [33,34] which seems to identify individuals with more severe self-injury phenotypes. For example Dulit et al. [35], studied a large group of patients with borderline personality disorder and those who reported 5 self-injury events in one year were more likely to have another psychiatric disorder, these findings have been confirmed in later studies [36,37], but opposing results have also been reported by Muehlenkamp et al. [38], who, in a clinical study of adolescents, proposed raising the frequency threshold of the DSM-5 criteria arguing that 5 events per year over-pathologize the behavior, an opinion shared by other researchers [39,40]. Further research comparing clinical and community samples is necessary.

We also found that adolescents who performed tattoos to feel pain chose different methods of self-injury (inserting objects under the skin and nails, using objects to mark the skin, burning) compared to adolescents without tattoos who selfinjure (scratching, cutting, rubbing and picking the skin) reported by other researchers [41-44]. Similar results were reported by Iannaccone et al. [45], who found a high correlation between burns and tattoos in women with eating behavior disorder.

Topography

The NSSI (T+) group was more likely to self-injure in all parts of the body involving buttocks/genitals, face, and head. Contrasting with the NSSI(T-) group in which forearms and wrists were preferred body locations, which is consistent with other research [41,43,46,47].

Although research is lacking, clinical experience suggests that self-injuring in face, breasts, eyes, and genitals is of special concern because it implies a higher level of psychological distress [48].

Dimensions B and C

In comparison to the NSSI (T-) adolescents in the NSSI (T+) group met the majority of DSM-5 criteria included in dimension B. B1: Release of negative feelings or cognitions; B2: Resolution of interpersonal difficulties; B3: Induction of positive feelings. Likewise, concerning Dimension C, the NSSI (T+) group reported higher frequencies of negative thoughts and feelings; which is similar to the findings reported by Aizenman and Jensen [16], who found tattooed college students who also self-injure had higher depression, lower self-esteem and mastery/control scores.

Addictive component (Table 3)

Compared to the NSSI (T-) group, the NSSI (T+) showed a higher frequency of addictive component (10.2% vs. 50% respectively) which was investigated through items 26–28 (Table 6). This is congruent with previous clinical research where addictive features are present in 10–97% of adolescents who practice NSSI [49,50]; and also with a review by Bunderla and Gregorič [51], who reported 5 studies showing higher pain threshold and longer pain tolerance in NSSI subjects. These results are consistent with a study of young college students, where 48.5% reported addictive features to tattooing behavior [11].

Non-suicide self-injury schedule

Dimension E social dysfunction

It is difficult to contrast our results with the few studies that have investigated the DSM-5 criteria in adolescents with NSSI. Increased absenteeism and dropout rates among tattooed adolescents are common [2,3,9,15,51]. These findings support school dysfunction, however, in a Mexican with young adults, Benjet et al. [52] suggested that individuals may not perceive this behavior as dysfunctional because it is used as an effective strategy to deal with unpleasant feelings, cognitions and interpersonal or family difficulties. In our sample, impairment was less frequent among those who self-injure without tattoos (18.3% compared to 39.9 of the Mexican sample of young adults) [52]. These differences could suggest adolescents have less awareness or normalize the behavior due to peer pressure or a desire for group integration. As Benjet et al. [52] points out, low perception of impairment is an obstacle for seeking treatment.

Severity indicators

Compared to the NSSI (T-) group, the NSSI (T+) group presented other indicators of self- injury severity, such as using more than one method, more anatomical sites, more frequent events and a greater propensity to suicide intent (87.5%); this is consistent with other studies that show teens and young adults with tattoos are at increased risk of suicide attempts and unhealthy practices [5,7,9,18,26,32,53,54]. However, the evidence is conflicting because other authors do not report this association [11,55]. Some researchers show tattoos could be a protective or mitigating factor for suicidal ideation and attempts [27]. Moreover, there is evidence of a decline and extinction of self-injurious behavior after tattooing [18] and most tattooed teens do not harm themselves, only 33% show selfinjury practices [8]. More research is needed to better characterize which individuals are more prone to engaging in suicidal behavior and other risk factors.

We also found that a high percentage of tattooed adolescents with NSSI have more suicide intent suggesting that suicide intent and NSSI are overlapping and therefore extremely difficult to differentiate due to its alternating nature, this explains why studies show that adolescents with NSSI are at higher risk of suicide [56].

The significance of our results is that health personnel and adults involved with the care of children and adolescents with tattoos should intentionally inquire about self-injurious behavior, and if present, investigate about risk behavior such as low academic achievement, unprotected sex, substance abuse, promiscuity, eating disorders, accidents and suicidal intent [4,5,9,15,18,45,54].

Limitations

The presence of tattoos was investigated through self- report and no visual confirmation was performed as some studies have done in clinical samples where the context is adequate for a physical examination.

We did not ask if the teenagers had requested permission from their parents prior to the tattooing process. There is evidence that around 83% of adolescents do not have permission from their parents to obtain a tattoo [57].

In Mexico, the practice of tattooing is prohibited in minors under 18 years of age, unless they are accompanied by their parents or guardians; after signing an informed consent letter, and a prior sanitary questionnaire and letter of authorization are completed [38]. Some studies show that more than 60% of adolescents do not ask their parents for permission to get a tattoo [27,57,58], although others report opposite findings [2,51] this is important because adolescents who do not ask for permission could also engage in more risky behavior than their peers who do not get tattoos or do so with proper authorization. Also, our small sample size precludes us from analyzing adolescents who tattoo themselves without the intention to feel pain and who engage in NSSI.

Conclusions

Our results support the idea that tattoos performed for feeling pain are a correlate of non-suicidal self-injury behavior, since they share DSM-5 characteristics, as frequency, and greater severity in most of variables. Hence the importance of asking the purpose behind getting a tattoo, whether they practice self-harm and assessing suicidal intentionality. Our findings also suggest that a group of individuals could engage in tattooing as a form of NSSI and further research could warrant adding a specifier in Diagnostic Criteria.

Acknowledgements

We the authors are grateful to all the families who participated in this study.

Notes

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Lilia Albores-Gallo. Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology: Marco Antonio Solís-Bravo, Yassel FloresRodríguez, Liliana Guadalupe Tapia-Guillen, Aymara Gatica-Hernández, Miriam Guzmán-Reséndiz, Lilia Albores-Gallo. Project administration: Lilia Albores-Gallo. Writing—original draft: Marco Antonio Solís-Bravo, Yassel Flores-Rodríguez, Liliana Guadalupe Tapia-Guillen. Writing—review & editing: Aymara Gatica-Hernández, Miriam Guzmán-Reséndiz, Luis Alberto Salinas-Torres, Tania Lucila Vargas-Rizo.

References

1. Serup J, Kluger N, Baumler W. Current Problems in Dermatology. 1st Edition. Epidemiology of Tattoos in Industrialized Countries. In : Kluger N, ed. Tattooed Skin and Health Basel: Karger Publishers; 2015. p. 6–20.
2. Busaniche JN, Eymann AM, Mulli V, Paz MC, Catsicaris C. Asociación entre adolescentes tatuados y conductas de riesgo. Arch Argent Pediatr 2006;104:309–315.
3. Farrow JA. Tattooing behavior in adolescence: a comparison study. Am J Dis Child 1991;145:184–187.
4. Roberts TA, Ryan SA. Tattooing and high-risk behavior in adolescents. Pediatrics 2002;110:1058–1063.
5. Carroll ST, Riffenburgh RH, Roberts TA, Myhre EB. Tattoos and body piercings as indicators of adolescent risk-taking behaviors. Pediatrics 2002;109:1021–1027.
6. Bicca JF, Duquia RP, Breunig J de A, Souza PR, Almeida HL Jr. Tattoos on 18-year-old male adolescents-characteristics and associated factors. An Bras Dermatol 2013;88:925–928.
7. Koch JR, Roberts AE, Armstrong ML, Owen DC. Tattoos, gender, and well-being among American college students. Soc Sci J 2015;52:536–541.
8. D’Ambrosio A, Casillo N, Martini V. Piercings and tattoos: psychopathological aspects. Act Nerv Super Rediviva 2013;55:143–148.
9. Deschesnes M, Finès P, Demers S. Are tattooing and body piercing indicators of risk-taking behaviours among high school students? J Adolesc 2006;29:379–393.
10. Dukes RL. Deviant ink: a meta-analysis of tattoos and drug use in general populations. Deviant Behav 2016;37:665–678.
11. King KA, Vidourek RA. Getting inked: tattoo and risky behavioral involvement among university students. Soc Sci J 2013;50:540–546.
12. Koch JR, Roberts AE, Armstrong ML, Owen DC. Body art, deviance, and American college students. Soc Sci J 2010;47:151–161.
13. Koch JR, Roberts AE, Armstrong ML, Owen DC. College students, tattoos, and sexual activity. Psychol Rep 2005;97:887–890.
14. Ammerman BA, Jacobucci R, Kleiman EM, Uyeji LL, McCloskey MS. The relationship between nonsuicidal self-injury age of onset and severity of self-harm. Suicide Life Threat Behav 2017;48:31–37.
15. Dhossche D, Snell KS, Larder S. A case-control study of tattoos in young suicide victims as a possible marker of risk. J Affect Disord 2000;59:165–168.
16. Aizenman M, Jensen MA. Speaking through the body: the incidence of self-injury, piercing, and tattooing among college students. J Coll Couns 2007;10:27–43.
17. Stirn A, Hinz A. Tattoos, body piercings, and self-injury: Is there a connection? Investigations on a core group of participants practicing body modification. Psychother Res 2008;18:326–333.
18. Stirn A, Hinz A, Brahler E. Prevalence of tattooing and body piercing in Germany and perception of health, mental disorders, and sensation seeking among tattooed and body- pierced individuals. J Psychosom Res 2006;60:531–534.
19. Dukes RL, Stein JA. Ink and Holes: correlates and predictive associations of body modification among adolescents. Youth Soc 2011;43:1547–1569.
20. Silver E, VanEseltine M, Silver SJ. Tattoo acquisition: a prospective study of adolescents. Deviant Behav 2009;30:511–538.
21. Favazza AR, Rosenthal RJ. Varieties of pathological self-mutilation. Behav Neurol 1990;3:77–85.
22. Favazza AR, Rosenthal RJ. Diagnostic issues in self-mutilation. Psychiatr Serv 1993;44:134–140.
23. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) 5th Edth ed. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
24. Emiliano-Altamirano I, Albores-Gallo L. “Validación de una cédula diagnóstica de autolesiones en un hospital psiquiátrico infantil de la Ciudad de México”. [Especialidad de Psiquiatría infantil y de la Adolescencia] Ciudad de México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México; 2014.
25. Cossio T ML, Giesen FL, Araya G, Pérez-Cotapos S ML. Association between tattoos, piercings and risk behaviors in adolescents. Rev Médica Chile 2012;140:198–206.
26. Yen CF, Hsiao RC, Yen JY, Yeh YC, Wang PW, Lin HC, et al. Tattooing among high school students in southern Taiwan: the prevalence, correlates and associations with risk-taking behaviors and depression. Kaohsiung J Med Sci 2012;28:383–389.
27. Claes L, Vandereycken W, Vertommen H. Self-care versus self-harm: piercing, tattooing, and self-injuring in eating disorders. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2005;13:11–28.
28. Armstrong ML, Owen DC, Roberts AE, Koch JR. College students and tattoos: Influence of image, identity, family, and friends. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv 2002;40:20–29.
29. Burger TD, Finkel D. Relationships between body modifications and very high-risk behavior in a college population. Coll Stud J 2002;36:203–214.
30. Drews DR, Allison CK, Probst JR. Behavioral and self-concept differences in tattooed and nontattooed college students. Psychol Rep 2000;86:475–481.
31. Hill BM, Ogletree SM, McCrary KM. Body modifications in college students: Considering gender, self-esteem, body appreciation, and reasons for tattoos. Coll Stud J 2016;50:246–252.
32. Owen DC, Armstrong ML, Koch JR, Roberts AE. College students with body art. Well-being or risk behavior. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv 2013;51:20–28.
33. Shaffer D, Jacobson C. Proposal to the DSM-V childhood disorder and mood disorder work groups to include non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) as a DSM-V disorder. Am Psychiatr Assoc 2009;:1–21.
34. Shaffer D, Colleen J. Proposal to the DSM-V Childhood Disorder and Mood Disorder Work Groups to Include Non-Suicidal Self-Injury (NSSI) as a DSM-V Disorder Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2010.
35. Dulit RA, Fyer MR, Leon AC, Brodsky BS, Frances AJ. Clinical correlates of self-mutilation in borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1994;151:1305–1311.
36. Matsumoto T, Yamaguchi A, Chiba Y, Asami T, Iseki E, Hirayasu Y. Patterns of self-cutting: a preliminary study on differences in clinical implications between wrist- and arm-cutting using a Japanese juvenile detention center sample. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2004;58:377–382.
37. Brunner R, Parzer P, Haffner J, Steen R, Roos J, Klett M, et al. Prevalence and psychological correlates of occasional and repetitive deliberate selfharm in adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2007;161:641–649.
38. Muehlenkamp JJ, Brausch AM, Washburn JJ. How much is enough? Examining frequency criteria for NSSI disorder in adolescent inpatients. J Consult Clin Psychol 2017;85:611–619.
39. Selby EA, Kranzler A, Fehling KB, Panza E. Nonsuicidal self-injury disorder: the path to diagnostic validity and final obstacles. Clin Psychol Rev 2015;38:79–91.
40. Brausch AM, Muehlenkamp JJ, Washburn JJ. Nonsuicidal self-injury disorder: does Criterion B add diagnostic utility? Psychiatry Res 2016;244:179–184.
41. Albores-Gallo L, Méndez-Santos JL, Xochitl-García Luna A, Delgadillo-Gonzalez Y, Chávez-Flores CI, Martínez OL. Nonsuicidal self-injury in a community sample of older children and adolescents of México City. Actas Esp Psiquiatr 2014;42:159–168.
42. Barrocas AL, Hankin BL, Young JF, Abela JRZ. Rates of nonsuicidal self-injury in youth: age, sex, and behavioral methods in a community sample. Pediatrics 2012;130:39–45.
43. Sornberger MJ, Heath NL, Toste JR, McLouth R. Nonsuicidal self-injury and gender: patterns of prevalence, methods, and locations among adolescents. Suicide Life Threat Behav 2012;42:266–278.
44. Castro Silva E, Benjet C, Juárez García F, Jurado Cárdenas S, Lucio Gómez-Maqueo ME, Valencia Cruz A, et al. Non-suicidal self-injuries in a sample of Mexican university students. Salud Ment 2017;40:191–200.
45. Iannaccone M, Cella S, Manzi SA, Visconti L, Manzi F, Cotrufo P. My body and me: self-injurious behaviors and body modifications in eating disorders-- preliminary results. Eat Disord 2013;21:130–139.
46. Victor SE, Muehlenkamp JJ, Hayes NA, Lengel GJ, Styer DM, Washburn JJ. Characterizing gender differences in nonsuicidal self-injury: evidence from a large clinical sample of adolescents and adults. Compr Psychiatry 2018;82:53–60.
47. Whitlock J, Eckenrode J, Silverman D. Self-injurious behaviors in a college population. Pediatrics 2006;117:1939–1948.
48. Whitlock J. Self-injurious behavior in adolescents. PLoS Med 2010;7e1000240.
49. Csorba J, Dinya E, Plener P, Nagy E, Páli E. Clinical diagnoses, characteristics of risk behaviour, differences between suicidal and non-suicidal subgroups of Hungarian adolescent outpatients practising selfinjury. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009;18:309–320.
50. Nixon MK, Cloutier PF, Aggarwal S. Affect regulation and addictive aspects of repetitive self-injury in hospitalized adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002;41:1333–1341.
51. Bunderla T, Gregorič H. Altered pain perception in self-injurious behavior and the association of psychological elements with pain perception measures: a systematic review. Psychiatr Danub 2015;27:346–354.
52. Benjet C, González-Herrera I, Castro-Silva E, Méndez E, Borges G, Casanova L, et al. Non-suicidal self-injury in Mexican young adults: Prevalence, associations with suicidal behavior and psychiatric disorders, and DSM-5 proposed diagnostic criteria. J Affect Disord 2017;215:1–8.
53. Ekinci O, Topcuoglu V, Sabuncuoglu O, Berkem M, Akin E, Gumustas FO. The association of tattooing/body piercing and psychopathology in adolescents: a community based study from Istanbul. Community Ment Health J 2012;48:798–803.
54. Hicinbothem J, Gonsalves S, Lester D. Body modification and suicidal behavior. Death Stud 2006;30:351–363.
55. Brooks TL, Woods ER, Knight JR, Shrier LA. Body modification and substance use in adolescents: is there a link? J Adolesc Health 2003;32:44–49.
56. Stephens MB. Behavioral risks associated with tattooing. Fam Med 2003;35:52–54.
57. Armstrong ML, Murphy KP. Tattooing: another adolescent risk behavior warranting health education. Appl Nurs Res 1997;10:181–189.
58. Groschwitz RC, Kaess M, Fischer G, Ameis N, Schulze UME, Brunner R, et al. The association of non-suicidal self-injury and suicidal behavior according to DSM-5 in adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Psychiatry Res 2015;228:454–461.

Article information Continued

Figure 1.

Frequency of events by group.

Table 1.

Characteristics of NSSI

Variables Tattoos to feel pain
OR (95% CI) χ2 p
No (%) Yes (%)
Method
 Cutting 9.9 50.0 9.1 (2.2–37.8) 13.31 0.006
 Rubbing 11.0 62.5 13.4 (3.1–58.0) 19.72 0.001
 Inserting objects under the skin and nails 5.4 62.5 29 (6.5–129.1) 42.19 0.0001
 Picking 16.0 50.0 5.2 (1.2–21.4) 6.52 0.029
 Using objects to mark the skin 8.9 62.5 17 (3.9–73.9) 25.25 0.0001
 Scratching 26.7 37.5 1.6 (0.38–6.9) 0.46 0.369
 Pulling the skin that peels after sunburn 19.1 37.5 2.5 (0.59–10.8) 1.70 0.188
 Burning the skin with cigarettes or corrosive substances 3.5 28.6 10.9 (1.9–60.8) 11.39 0.027
 Plucking hair, eyelashes or eyebrows? (excluding cosmetic reasons) 4.7 25.0 6.7 (1.2–35.4) 6.68 0.058
 Ripping off nails 6.8 25.0 4.5 (0.87–23.5) 3.88 0.106
 Biting 16.7 37.5 2.9 (0.69–12.7) 2.38 0.142
 Hitting body or head or slapping 11.1 50.0 8.0 (1.9–33.2) 11.45 0.008
Topography
 Forearms and wrists 7.5 42.9 9.1 (1.9–42.8) 11.50 0.001
 Arms and elbows 3.6 50.0 26.9 (6.1–118.1) 39.78 0.0001
 Thighs and knees 4.2 42.9 17.0 (3.5–81.6) 22.27 0.003
 Abdomen and thorax (chest or back) 2.8 42.9 25.8 (5.2–128.5) 33.01 0.001
 Face 3.3 75.0 87.8 (16.2–474.5) 91.41 0.0001
 Head 3.3 50.0 29.4 (6.6–129.8) 43.10 0.0001
 Buttocks or genitals 1.2 75.0 252.6 (40.6–1570) 173.25 0.0001

NSSI: non suicidal self injury, OR: odds ratio, CI: confidence interval

Table 2.

Psychological precipitants of NSSI

Variables Tattoos to feel pain
OR (95% CI) χ2 p
No (%) Yes (%)
Feelings and cognitive states before NSSI
 Negative feelings or thoughts 12.7 62.5 11.4 (2.6–49.2) 16.54 0.002
 Depression 9.9 62.5 15.1 (3.5–65.8) 22.46 0.001
 Anxiety 5.9 71.4 40.1 (7.4–217.0) 45.90 0.0001
 Tension 5.4 62.5 29.2 (6.5–129.8) 42.42 0.0001
 Anger 10.3 62.5 14.4 (3.3–62.6) 21.34 0.001
 Stress 6.6 62.5 23.6 (5.3–104.2) 34.96 0.0001
 Self-criticism 7.3 75.0 38.2 (7.4–197.3) 46.18 0.0001
 Resistance to perform this behavior 8.3 50.0 11.1 (2.6–46.3) 16.66 0.003
Feelings and cognitive states during NSSI
 Preoccupation about the behavior 12.3 37.5 4.2 (0.99–18.4) 4.50 0.069
 Relief of tension 5.4 25.0 5.8 (1.1–30.5) 5.55 0.072
Feelings and cognitive states after NSSI
 Preoccupation about the behavior 14.4 50.0 5.9 (1.4–24.3) 7.76 0.021
 Relief of tension immediately after 6.6 37.5 8 (1.9–37.2) 11.21 0.015
Motivations for NSSI
 Relief from negative feelings 8.5 50.0 10.7 (2.5–44.9) 16.10 0.003
 Relief from negative thoughts 7.3 37.5 7.6 (1.7–33.4) 9.90 0.019
 Relief from personal difficulties 6.6 50.0 14.1 (3.3–59.5) 21.55 0.001
 Induce positive feelings 3.1 37.5 19.0 (4.1–88.1) 26.17 0.002
 Need to perform the act frequently (even if not always executed) 7.1 25.0 4.3 (0.84–22.6) 3.69 0.112

NSSI: non suicidal self injury, OR: odds ratio, CI: confidence interval

Table 3.

Addictive component of NSSI

Variables Tattoos to feel pain
OR (95% CI) χ2 p
No (%) Yes (%)
Feeling they can’t stop self-injuring 10.2 50.0 8.7 (2.1–36.3) 12.66 0.006
Feelings of addiction to self-injuring 4.3 71.4 55.1 (10.0–303.7) 60.13 0.0001
Desire to stop self-injuring 15.5 57.1 7.2 (1.5–33.1) 8.76 0.015

NSSI: non suicidal self injury, OR: odds ratio, CI: confidence interval

Table 4.

NSSI dimensions

Dimension Tattoos to feel pain
OR (95% CI) χ2 p
No (%) Yes (%)
A Frequency of self-injury 46.9 87.5 7.9 (1–64.8) 5.17 0.030
B Contingent response to self-injury
 B1 Relief from negative feelings (-) 9.9 62.5 15.2 (3.5–66.0) 22.53 0.001
 B2 Resolution Interpersonal Difficulties 6.6 50.9 14.1 (3.3–59.5) 21.55 0.001
 B3 Induction of Positive Feelings 3.1 37.5 19.0 (4.1–88.1) 26.1 0.002
C Affects and Cognitions Associated to NSSI
 C1 Interpersonal difficulties or negative feelings or thoughts 17.1 87.5 33.8 (4.1–279.3) 25.85 0.0001
 C2 Period of preoccupation 8.3 50.0 11.1 (2.6–46.3) 16.6 0.003
 C3 NSSI Rumination 7.1 25.0 4.3 (0.849–22.6) 3.69 0.112
E Impairment
 E1 Interpersonal 5.6 57.1 22.3 (4.7–105.4) 30.2 0.0001
 E2 Academic 3.1 75 95.3 (17.5–517.9) F 0.0001
 E3 Other areas 3.3 50 29.2 (6.6–128.9) F 0.0001

NSSI: non suicidal self injury, OR: odds ratio, CI: confidence interval

Table 5.

Suicidal Intent

Variables Tattoos to feel pain
OR (95% CI) χ2 p
No (%) Yes (%)
Self-injuries with the intention to die 2.1 87.5 323.5 (35.9–2911.0) 160.8 0.0001
Self-injuries without the intention to die 10.6 62.5 14 (3.2–60.5) 20.5 0.001
Suicidal thoughts when self-injuring without the intention to die 4.9 50.0 19.2 (4.4–82.3) 29.3 0.0001

OR: odds ratio, CI: confidence interval

Table 6.

Non-suicide self-injury schedule

Item Question
1 Do you hurt yourself without the intention to die?
2 In the last month, have you hurt yourself one to three times?
3 In the last 6 months, have you hurt yourself one to three times?
4 In the last year, have you hurt yourself five or more times?
5 Have you cut your skin?
6 Have you rubbed your skin with an object?
7 Have you inserted any object under your skin or nails?
8 Have you picked your skin?
9 Have you ever used any object (pencil, compass, needle, cutter, others...) to mark your skin?
10 Have you scratched scars?
11 Have you pulled the skin that peels after sunburn?
12 Have burned your skin with cigarettes or corrosive substances?
13 Have you plucked your hair, eyebrows or eyelashes (excluding cosmetic reasons)?
14 Have you ripped off your nails?
15 Have you bitten yourself?
16 Have you hit your body or head or slapped yourself?
17 Do you perform these acts to hurt yourself because they release you from negative feelings?
18 Do you perform these acts to hurt yourself because they release you from negative thoughts?
19 Do you perform these acts to hurt yourself because they release you from personal difficulties?
20 Do you perform these acts to hurt yourself because they induce positive feelings?
21 Do you feel a relief of tension during the act of harming yourself?
22 Do you feel a relief of tension immediately after the act of harming yourself?
23 Does the act of self-injuring interfere with your academic duties?
24 Does the act of self-injuring interfere with your rest or sleep?
25 Does the act of self-injuring interfere with your interpersonal relationships?
26 Do you feel you can’t stop self-injuring?
27 Do you feel you’re addicted to self-injuring?
28 Do you want to stop self-injuring?
29 Do you hurt yourself with the intention to die?
30 When you hurt yourself without the intention to die, do you have suicidal thoughts?
31 Did you ever get a tattoo to feel pain?